The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective:

Adolescents and young adults without childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often present to clinics seeking stimulant medication for late-onset ADHD symptoms. Recent birth-cohort studies support the notion of late-onset ADHD, but these investigations are limited by relying on screening instruments to assess ADHD, not considering alternative causes of symptoms, or failing to obtain complete psychiatric histories. The authors address these limitations by examining psychiatric assessments administered longitudinally to the local normative comparison group of the Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD.

Method:

Individuals without childhood ADHD (N=239) were administered eight assessments from comparison baseline (mean age=9.89 years) to young adulthood (mean age=24.40 years). Diagnostic procedures utilized parent, teacher, and self-reports of ADHD symptoms, impairment, substance use, and other mental disorders, with consideration of symptom context and timing.

Results:

Approximately 95% of individuals who initially screened positive on symptom checklists were excluded from late-onset ADHD diagnosis. Among individuals with impairing late-onset ADHD symptoms, the most common reason for diagnostic exclusion was symptoms or impairment occurring exclusively in the context of heavy substance use. Most late-onset cases displayed onset in adolescence and an adolescence-limited presentation. There was no evidence for adult-onset ADHD independent of a complex psychiatric history.

Conclusions:

Individuals seeking treatment for late-onset ADHD may be valid cases; however, more commonly, symptoms represent nonimpairing cognitive fluctuations, a comorbid disorder, or the cognitive effects of substance use. False positive late-onset ADHD cases are common without careful assessment. Clinicians should carefully assess impairment, psychiatric history, and substance use before treating potential late-onset cases.

In recent years, an influx of adolescents and young adults without documented childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have presented to clinics with complaints of inattention and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms, often inquiring about stimulant medication (13). It remains unclear whether this trend is driven by typically developing individuals seeking stimulant medication for cognitive enhancement or by individuals with late-onset ADHD that warrants medical treatment. Recent birth cohort studies support the phenomenon of late-onset ADHD, reporting a 2.5%−10.7% prevalence for a form of ADHD that first emerges in adolescence or adulthood (47). These studies claim that most adult ADHD cases (67.5%−90.0%) do not involve the experience of symptom onset in childhood. This claim is contrary to decades of research characterizing ADHD as a chronic neurodevelopmental disorder with symptoms that appear before age 12 (811). The authors speculate that late-onset ADHD may appear spontaneously, but critics suggest that these cases may also represent individuals with undetected childhood symptoms (i.e., late-identified rather than late-onset) (1214).

Critics also suggest that late-onset ADHD prevalence may be inflated by methodological artifacts, such as reliance on ADHD screening instruments, inability to detect symptoms that emerged in long gaps between assessments, a false-positive paradox, and failure to consider other mental disorders, health problems, or substance abuse as the source of symptoms (1214). If many late-onset cases are false positives, this may misinform the field’s understanding of ADHD as a chronic disorder and overstate its prevalence. On the other hand, true late-onset ADHD may partially explain the uptick in adolescents and young adults seeking first-time treatment for newly reported difficulties (47).

The present study investigates late-onset ADHD in the local normative comparison group of the Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD, which was designed to carefully assess ADHD symptoms over time (15, 16). For 14 years from childhood to adulthood, comparison participants underwent comprehensive psychiatric evaluations with multi-informant assessment of ADHD symptoms and impairments (17, 18). Due to the frequency (eight time points) and comprehensiveness of these assessments, ADHD symptom onset, other mental disorders, impairments, and substance use can be isolated temporally and considered when determining the history and nature of potential late-onset cases. Through careful review of multi-informant, longitudinal psychiatric data using a stepped diagnostic procedure that pinpoints symptom origins‎, we aimed to 1) understand what proportion of individuals with reported late-onset ADHD symptoms represent true cases of the disorder and 2) provide detailed clinical profiles for identified late-onset ADHD cases. Our procedure complements the epidemiological population studies by exploring the nature of late-onset ADHD after addressing previously noted methodological confounds and illustrating how late-onset ADHD might emerge over time (1214).

Method

The Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD compared effects of 14 months of pharmacological and psychosocial treatments for children (7.0–9.9 years old) with ADHD, combined type (15). Two years after baseline, 289 classmates were recruited for the local normative comparison group. The Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD continued with prospective follow-up until 16 years after baseline (1518). Informed consent was obtained in childhood and adulthood.

Participants

We identified a comparison group subsample (N=239) (Table 1) who did not meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD during childhood baseline assessment and who had at least one assessment in adolescence (ages 12–17) and adulthood (aged 18 years or older). Of the 289 originally recruited comparison participants, we excluded 31 individuals with a baseline Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children diagnosis of ADHD (1719) and 19 participants with insufficient follow-up data. This subsample (N=239) was recruited between 8.19 and 13.85 years of age (mean=9.89 years [SD=1.22]), and the average age at the final adult assessment was 24.40 years (SD=1.36).

TABLE 1. Baseline Characteristics of the Comparison Subsample (N=239)a

Characteristic
N%
Male sex19179.9
Race/ethnicity
 White15966.5
 Black2711.3
 Hispanic3112.9
 Other229.3
MeanSD
Age at baseline (years)9.891.22
Intelligence scoreb109.8218.65
SNAP Rating Scale scorec
 Baseline inattention symptoms count1.702.61
 Baseline hyperactivity/impulsivity symptom count1.031.92

aThe median household income of the comparison subsample was $55,000.

bDetermined using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-3rd edition.

cSNAP=Swanson, Nolan and Pelham Rating Scale. (SNAP average severity scores range from 0 [“not at all”] to 3 [“very much”].)

TABLE 1. Baseline Characteristics of the Comparison Subsample (N=239)a

Enlarge table

Procedures

Comparison group recruitment was designed to reflect the local population from which the ADHD sample was drawn. Classes in the schools of the ADHD participants were randomly selected. After obtaining consent from more than 50% of the classmates in the selected classroom, individuals were selected randomly and group-matched for sex. ADHD diagnosis was neither inclusionary nor exclusionary for the comparison group. Study assessments were administered to comparison participants upon recruitment (comparison baseline; 2 years after ADHD baseline) and at 3, 6, 8, 10, 12, 14, and 16 years after initial baseline by bachelor’s-level staff who were trained to be objective.

Measures

ADHD symptoms.

Symptoms in childhood and adolescence were measured using the SNAP [Swanson, Nolan and Pelham] Rating Scale completed by parents, teachers, and adolescents (20, 21). Symptoms in adulthood were measured using the Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale completed by participants and parents (22). The SNAP and Conners’ scales both list DSM-IV-TR ADHD symptoms. Respondents indicated the extent to which participants displayed each symptom on a scale from 0 “not at all” to 3 “very much.” Scores of 2 and 3 indicated symptom presence, as is standard practice when using these scales to detect clinically meaningful ADHD symptoms (23).

Impairment.

In adolescence, impairment was measured using the parent version of the Columbia Impairment Scale (24). Because the Columbia Impairment Scale assesses impairment across multiple domains, including several that are unrelated to ADHD (e.g., feeling nervous/afraid), we examined impairment scores for four central domains of ADHD-related impairment: “getting along with kids own age,” “schoolwork,” “behavior at home,” and “behavior at school.” The scale utilizes a 0–4 severity range, and a score ≥3 in at least one of the four domains was considered sufficient to meet the impairment threshold (25). In adulthood, parent- and self-versions of the Impairment Rating Scale were used to measure impairment globally and in 11 domains of functioning (26). Response options ranged from 0 (“no problem”) to 6 (“extreme problem”). The Impairment Rating Scale is a measure of general impairment and has strong psychometric properties for identifying ADHD-related impairment. An empirically validated cutoff score >3 on any item was used to define clinically significant impairment (26).

Substance use.

Heavy substance use was measured using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children and Substance Use Questionnaire (19, 27, 28). Substance use disorders reported on the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children by either the parent or participant were considered when determining late-onset ADHD. Self-reported marijuana or other drug use on the Substance Use Questionnaire more than twice per week was classified as heavy substance use.

Mental disorders.

On the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (19), parent- or self-report that indicated the presence of a mental disorder that better accounted for ADHD symptoms was exclusionary for a late-onset ADHD diagnosis. All disorders assessed using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children were considered (see the data supplement accompanying the online version of this article). Eight experienced, licensed clinicians (three psychiatrists, five clinical psychologists) reviewed onset and chronicity of all mental symptoms, and each voted whether a case should be excluded based on ADHD symptoms or impairment being attributable to another disorder (e.g., effects of anxiety symptoms on concentration). A case was excluded if agreed upon by a majority. Most decisions were unanimous (see the online data supplement).

Analytic Plan

There is a considerable risk for both false negative and false positive ADHD diagnoses in adolescents and adults (29). Regarding false negatives, there is established underreporting of ADHD symptoms in non-self-referred children, adolescents, and adults, concern that informants do not fully observe the functioning of adolescents and adults, and evidence that wording of some DSM ADHD symptoms may not be developmentally relevant for adolescents and adults (21, 2932). Regarding false positives, normative variations in attention can be mistaken for ADHD symptoms, and ADHD symptoms often overlap with features of other disorders (33). To optimize sensitivity and specificity, our strategy to assess adolescent- and adult-onset ADHD took the stepped approach outlined by Sibley et al. (34), which first casts an intentionally wide net for ADHD symptoms to protect against false negatives (using a version of an “or rule” that allows all reported symptoms to be considered). The second step protects against false positives by carefully assessing and requiring meaningful impairment, establishing symptoms across settings, and ruling out substance abuse or other mental disorders as the source of ADHD-like symptoms.

Symptom criteria.

At each assessment, ratings on the SNAP (parent, teacher, and adolescent) or Conners’ (parent and adult) scale were combined at the item-level using an “or rule,” such that if a symptom was endorsed by any rater, it was deemed present. Symptom count was determined separately for inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. After calculating combined symptom count, DSM-5 symptom thresholds were applied considering current age (six symptoms for participants ages 12–16; five symptoms for participants aged 17 and over) for either inattention or hyperactivity/impulsivity (35).

Impairment.

Next, parent- and self-ratings from the Impairment Rating Scale were combined at the item level using an “or” rule to designate clinically significant impairment. If a participant who met symptom threshold for ADHD also had clinically significant impairment according to the parent Columbia Impairment Scale (adolescents) or combined Impairment Rating Scale (adults), he or she was retained as a potential case of late-onset ADHD.

Onset.

We examined SNAP symptom data at all assessments for those cases with symptoms and impairment in adolescence (ages 12–17) or adulthood (aged 18 or older). If a case subject was younger than 12 years old when symptom criteria for ADHD were first met, the individual was not considered to be late-onset.

Substance use.

All retained cases were examined to determine whether heavy substance use was a probable source of ADHD symptoms. If ADHD symptoms occurred exclusively in the context of heavy substance use, we designated substance use to be the source of ADHD symptoms.

Other mental disorders.

Next, retained cases were examined to determine whether ADHD symptoms or impairments were better explained by another mental disorder. Cases with comorbidities were retained as potential cases of late-onset ADHD if there was low likelihood that the comorbid disorder could account for ADHD symptoms or impairments.

Cross-situational symptoms.

DSM-5 ADHD diagnosis requires several symptoms to be present in two or more settings (35). Therefore, cross-situational symptoms were required at the time DSM-5 symptom thresholds were met. Cross-situational symptoms were defined as 1) at least two symptoms reported, each by the parent and teacher, or 2) at least two symptoms endorsed, each by the case subject (self-report) and another informant. Because symptoms endorsed on self-reports might occur in the same setting as parent or teacher reports, we consulted interview questions about symptom setting to ensure self-reported symptoms represented a second context.

Onset and chronicity.

Among case subjects who met criteria for late-onset ADHD, we calculated the average age at onset and examined chronicity by plotting ADHD symptoms by rater at each assessment point. To consider whether included case subjects were late-onset compared with late-identified, we compared childhood ADHD symptom severity for included cases to sample (N=239) means at baseline in childhood (see Table 1).

Results

Adolescent-Onset ADHD

An outline of the multistep assessment process and display of the proportion of case subjects included in each step are provided in Table 2.

TABLE 2. Results of Stepped Procedure for Evaluating the Validity of Late-Onset ADHD Casesa

ResultAdolescent-OnsetAdult-Onset
N%N%
Meets DSM-5 ADHD symptom criteria9640.24719.7
 Clinically significant impairment3213.44016.7
  Late-onset218.82410.0
   Not due to substance abuse187.5104.1
    Not attributable to other mental disorder135.431.3
     Cross-situational symptoms62.520.8
Absence of subthreshold childhood symptoms (less than three childhood symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity)31.32b0.8

aSymptom criteria were counted using an “or rule” that considered information from all available informants (e.g., parent, self, teacher); the designated period was either adolescence or adulthood; cross-situationality was inferred from multiple raters and consulting interview questions about context as needed.

bOne case subject was first assessed at age 12, at which point there were not subthreshold symptoms.

TABLE 2. Results of Stepped Procedure for Evaluating the Validity of Late-Onset ADHD Casesa

Enlarge table

Symptom criteria.

Of the 239 comparison case subjects without ADHD at baseline, 96 (40.2%) met DSM-5 symptom threshold for ADHD based on combined parent, teacher, and self-reports using an item level “or rule” during at least one adolescent follow-up assessment. (If a stricter “or rule” was applied requiring a single rater to endorse symptoms above the DSM-5 threshold, 93 adolescents met DSM-5 ADHD symptom count.)

Impairment.

Of the 96 case subjects who met symptom criteria for ADHD in adolescence, 32 (33.0%) experienced clinically significant impairment at the time they met the DSM-5 symptom count. In total, 13.4% of the 239 comparison case subjects without ADHD at baseline met both symptom and impairment criteria for ADHD at an adolescent follow-up assessment.

Adolescent-onset.

Among these 32 cases, 11 were under age 12 when they first met DSM ADHD symptom count according to at least one source and were considered childhood-onset cases. Thus, only 21 case subjects actually had onset during adolescence.

Ruling out substance use.

Among the 21 case subjects who showed adolescent-onset ADHD symptoms and impairment, three had a marijuana use disorder that better accounted for the ADHD symptoms. In total, 18 cases of adolescent-onset ADHD with significant impairment were not attributable to heavy substance use.

Ruling out other disorders.

Of these 18 case subjects, nine had a history of pre-existing or concurrent mental disorders and were reviewed by the clinical panel. The panel voted to exclude five based on evidence that symptoms better reflected another mental disorder (see the online data supplement). Thus, 13 case subjects appeared to have onset of elevated ADHD symptoms and impairment in adolescence that was not attributable to other mental disorders.

Cross-situational symptoms.

Of the 13 case subjects who had onset of elevated ADHD symptoms and impairment in adolescence, six had symptoms that were only reported by a teacher. One had symptoms that were reported by the teacher and the participant (self-report), but self-reported symptoms occurred only in the classroom. Thus, six case subjects (2.5% of the comparison subjects without ADHD at baseline) appeared to have an onset of elevated ADHD symptoms and impairment in adolescence that were present in more than one setting (see Table 2).

Onset and chronicity.

The average age at onset among the six adolescent-onset cases of ADHD was 14.22 years (SD=1.50; range: 12.09–16.08). The chronicity of ADHD across assessment points for all adolescent-onset ADHD cases is shown in Figures 1 and 2. Four of these six met symptom criteria only during the teenage years. These four remitting case subjects did not receive any medication or behavioral treatments for ADHD during the follow-up period. Two had symptoms that persisted into their 20s. Five of the six adolescent-onset case subjects (83.3%) had childhood ADHD symptoms that exceeded sample baseline means (see Table 1 and Figures 1 and 2). The average number of childhood symptoms among the six included case subjects was 2.5 for inattention (range=0–5; SD=2.26; Cohen’s d=0.31) and 1.67 for hyperactivity/impulsivity (range=0–3; SD=1.21; Cohen’s d=0.33).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Adolescence-Limited ADHD Cases: Symptom Counts According to Parent, Teacher, Self-, and Combined Reports at Each Available Assessment Pointa

a Symptoms in the shaded region exceed DSM-5 age-specific symptom thresholds. Childhood health and behavioral history was reported retrospectively at baseline. Substance use and mental health diagnoses were obtained from the parent- and self-reported Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. Bold lines represent combined report across raters using an “or rule.” For case B, symptom duration was assessed by consulting the self-reported Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. HI=hyperactivity/impulsivity; IN=inattention; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder; P=parent report; S=self-report; T=teacher report.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Adolescent-Onset Persistent ADHD Cases: Symptom Counts According to Parent, Teacher, Self-, and Combined Reports at Each Available Assessment Pointa

a Symptoms in the shaded region exceed DSM-5 age-specific symptom thresholds. Childhood health and behavioral history was reported retrospectively at baseline. Substance use and mental health diagnoses were obtained from the parent- and self-reported Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. Bold lines represent combined report across raters using an “or rule.” One voter dissented for the inclusion of case E. HI=hyperactivity/impulsivity; IN=inattention; OCD=obsessive-compulsive disorder; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder; P=parent report; S=self-report; T=teacher report.

Adult-Onset ADHD

Symptom criteria.

Of 239 comparison case subjects without ADHD at baseline, 19.7% (N=47) met DSM-5 symptom criteria for ADHD during at least one adult assessment based on combined parent and self-report using an item-level “or rule.” (If a stricter “or rule” was applied requiring a single rater to endorse symptoms above the DSM-5 threshold, 43 adults met DSM-5 ADHD symptom criteria.)

Impairment.

Among 47 case subjects who met symptom criteria, 40 (85.1%) experienced clinically significant impairment. In total, 16.7% of the 239 comparison cases without ADHD at baseline met both symptom and impairment criteria for ADHD during at least one adult assessment.

Adult onset.

Of the 40 case subjects with both ADHD symptoms and impairment in adulthood, 12 showed symptom onset during childhood, 18 during adolescence, and 10 during adulthood. Four were previously deemed adolescent-onset cases. Thus, 24 of 239 case subjects first met impairment criteria for ADHD in adulthood, although 14 had initial symptom onset in adolescence and 10 had initial symptom onset in adulthood.

Ruling out substance use.

Of the 24 case subjects meeting symptom and impairment criteria, 14 had impairing symptoms exclusively in the context of heavy substance use (see the online data supplement). In total, 10 adult-onset ADHD cases were not attributable to heavy substance use.

Ruling out other mental disorders.

Of the 10 remaining case subjects, five were excluded because symptoms or impairment were attributable to another mental health disorder. Two did not possess Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children data for adulthood, and these cases were deemed inconclusive. Thus, three case subjects appeared to have onset of elevated ADHD symptoms and impairment in adolescence that was not attributable to other mental disorders. One of the included adult case subjects was excluded in adolescence due to anxiety and mania but included in adulthood because comorbid disorders had remitted when ADHD symptoms returned (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Adult-Onset ADHD Cases: Symptom Counts According to Parent, Teacher, Self-, and Combined Reports at Each Available Assessment Pointa

a For case G, symptoms reported at age 13.46 and age 15.15 were deemed by a panel of clinical experts to be attributable to other mental disorders (anxiety disorders and mania). As a result, onset of symptoms that appear not to be attributable to other disorders occurs at 21.05 years. Symptoms in the shaded region exceed DSM-5 age-specific symptom thresholds. Childhood health and behavioral history was reported retrospectively at baseline. Substance use and mental health diagnoses were obtained from the parent- and self-reported Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. Bold lines represent combined report across raters using an “or rule.” Two voters dissented for the inclusion of case G based on symptom presence at age 21.05. For cases G and H, symptom duration was reported to be over 6 months on the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. GAD=generalized anxiety disorder; HI=hyperactivity/impulsivity; IN=inattention; OCD=obsessive-compulsive disorder; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder; P=parent report; PTSD=posttraumatic stress disorder; S=self-report; T=teacher report.

Cross-situational symptoms.

One of the three remaining adult-onset ADHD case subjects possessed symptoms in only one setting. Thus, of 239 comparison case subjects without ADHD at baseline, only two (0.8%) showed evidence of adult-onset ADHD (see Table 2).

Onset and chronicity.

The adult-onset case subjects reported onset at ages 21.05 and 27.45, respectively. Both met criteria for ADHD at only one adult assessment. One subject’s childhood symptoms (inattention, N=0; hyperactivity/impulsivity, N=1) were below the baseline sample average. The other was first assessed at age 12, reporting one inattention symptom and two hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms at that time (see Figure 3).

Characteristics of case subjects with late-onset ADHD symptoms and impairment who were excluded from diagnosis are summarized in the online data supplement.

Discussion

The local normative comparison group of the Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD provided a unique opportunity to study detailed fluctuations in ADHD symptoms over time in adolescents and young adults without a childhood history of ADHD. After using a stepped diagnostic procedure that carefully considered multi-informant data, longitudinal symptom patterns from childhood to adulthood, impairment, co-occurring mental disorders, and substance use, approximately 95% of case subjects who initially screened positive for late-onset ADHD were excluded from diagnosis (Table 2). These data indicate that when assessing adolescents and young adults for first-time ADHD diagnoses, clinicians should obtain a thorough psychiatric history and assessment of current functioning. Furthermore, 53% of adolescents and 83% of adults who met all symptom, impairment, and late-onset criteria for ADHD were excluded because symptoms or impairment were better explained by heavy substance use or another mental disorder (Table 2) (also see the online data supplement). Therefore, previously reported late-onset ADHD prevalence rates (2.5%−10.7%) may be overestimated due to limited ability to consult multi-informant data, track symptoms in extended gaps between assessment points, and review detailed patterns of substance use and comorbidity over time when determining diagnosis (47).

Six adolescent-onset ADHD case subjects appeared in the comparison group. One form of adolescent-onset ADHD (N=4) was adolescence-limited (Figure 1) and characterized by above-average childhood symptoms, borderline to average intelligence, and symptom remission by age 19. In all four of these cases, the preponderance of symptoms was reported by teachers, although corroborated by parents and the adolescents. One explanation for this pattern is developmental misfit that mimics or facilitates inattention symptoms. Mounting environmental demands in adolescence may temporarily exacerbate above-average but subthreshold childhood ADHD symptoms (Figure 1) or create cognitive overload for adolescents with slower developing prefrontal regions (36, 37). In absence of mature executive functions, some adolescents may also display deficient self-control in socially or emotionally salient contexts, leading to adolescence-limited behavior problems that may be perceived as hyperactive/impulsive symptoms by raters (38). Further work is needed to better understand this adolescence-limited presentation and the influence of cognitive development on ADHD-like symptoms in adolescents without childhood ADHD.

A second adolescent-onset ADHD presentation was characterized by above-average childhood ADHD symptoms and superior intellect (Figure 2). Two male subjects with superior IQs exhibited a persistent form of late-onset ADHD with slowly escalating symptoms from childhood through young adulthood. This profile echoes previous findings that childhood ADHD symptoms may be masked in individuals with cognitive strengths, delaying initial ADHD diagnosis (1). Since symptoms were likely present but mitigated in childhood, these individuals might better be characterized as late-identified, rather than late-onset, ADHD cases (39).

The Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD comparison group did not support adult-onset ADHD independent of a complex psychiatric history. The two case subjects identified as adult-onset both possessed a variety of past or current mental health symptoms (Figure 3). In both cases, it was difficult to disentangle the etiology of these individuals’ symptoms, and thus the panel conservatively voted to retain the cases. In line with the false-positive paradox (8), the vast majority of case subjects who initially met late-onset symptom and impairment criteria were excluded from diagnosis because of clear evidence that heavy substance use or another mental disorder better accounted for symptoms or impairment (Table 2). In fact, the majority of impairing late-onset ADHD symptoms in young adulthood could be traced to heavy substance use (Table 2) (also see the online data supplement). There are still other potential causes of late-onset symptoms, such as brain injury, illness, or trauma, that should also be considered in future investigations. Without clear exclusionary guidelines for ADHD in adolescents and adults, there is risk that ADHD may become a catchall diagnosis for executive dysfunction stemming from any source. It is unclear whether ADHD-like presentations stemming from nontraditional sources should be differentiated from a chronic form of ADHD with developmental origins, although treatment may be similar (40). Despite many strengths to birth-cohort samples, they are limited because they do not possess the detailed and frequent data collection required to carefully follow psychiatric functioning over time. One of the studies also did not perform full childhood diagnostic assessments, which may have led to missed childhood symptoms in some cases (5). Of course, the average age at comparison baseline was approximately 10 years old, limiting our study’s ability to consider detailed symptom records before this assessment.

The comparison group was drawn from the same local school, sex, and age/grade pool as the ADHD sample, which may over-represent certain characteristics, such as male sex or slightly above-average family income. During adolescence, impairment ratings were only available from parents. Some case subjects may have met impairment criteria in adolescence if teacher or self-ratings had been available. We assessed case subjects only to the mid-to-late 20s. New late-onset cases might appear later in development. We also did not collect comprehensive data on physical health or personality disorders with impulsive features that may better explain late-onset cases. Because only eight late-onset cases were detected, we were insufficiently powered to conduct analyses comparing late-onset cases with other subgroups.

Conclusions

Some adolescents and young adults who present for first-time ADHD diagnoses may represent valid late-onset cases. However, the most common source of impairing late-onset ADHD symptoms in adolescence and young adulthood was substance use. Prior to diagnosing or treating ADHD in late-onset cases, clinicians should carefully assess and treat substance use and comorbid mental health disorders as a potential source of symptoms. The majority of adolescent-onset cases possessed transient symptoms. Thus, it may be appropriate to give provisional first-time ADHD diagnoses in adolescence and to monitor symptoms over time, as remission may occur within a few years. Further research is needed to understand how cognitive immaturity or adolescent neurocognitive changes might mimic or facilitate emerging ADHD symptoms.

From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health, Florida International University, Miami; the Department of Psychiatry, Hospital de Clinicas de Porto Alegre, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil; the Child Development Center, School of Medicine, University of California, Irvine; the Division of Child Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal Children’s Hospital, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; the Departments of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Pediatrics, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, N.C.; the Department of Psychiatry, Ohio State University, Nisonger Center, Columbus, Ohio; the Division of Molecular Psychiatry, University Hospital Wuerzberg, Germany; and the Department of Pediatrics, University of California, Irvine.
Send correspondence to Dr. Sibley ().

Supported by cooperative agreement grants and contracts from NIMH and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) to the following: University of California-Berkeley: U01 MH50461, N01MH12009, and HHSN271200800005-C; DA-8-5550; Duke University: U01 MH50477, N01MH12012, and HHSN271200800009-C; DA-8-5554; University of California-Irvine: U01MH50440, N01MH12011, and HHSN271200800006-C; DA-8-5551; Research Foundation for Mental Hygiene (New York State Psychiatric Institute/Columbia University): U01 MH50467, N01 MH12007, and HHSN271200800007-C; DA-8-5552; Long Island-Jewish Medical Center U01 MH50453; New York University: N01MH 12004, and HHSN271200800004-C; DA-8-5549; University of Pittsburgh: U01 MH50467, N01 MH 12010, and HHSN271200800008-C; DA-8-5553; DA039881; and McGill University N01MH12008, and HHSN271200800003-C; DA-8-5548. Additional funding support was provided by NIDA (grant K23DA032577) to Dr. Mitchell.

The Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) was an NIMH cooperative agreement randomized clinical trial, continued under an NIMH contract as a follow-up study and finally under a NIDA contract. Collaborators from NIMH are as follows: Benedetto Vitiello, M.D. (Child and Adolescent Treatment and Preventive Interventions Research Branch), Joanne B. Severe, M.S. (Clinical Trials Operations and Biostatistics Unit, Division of Services and Intervention Research), Peter S. Jensen, M.D. (currently at REACH Institute and Mayo Clinic), L. Eugene Arnold, M.D., M.Ed. (currently at Ohio State University), Kimberly Hoagwood, Ph.D. (currently at Columbia). Previous contributors from NIMH to the early phases: John Richters, Ph.D. (currently at the National Institute of Nursing Research), Donald Vereen, M.D. (currently at NIDA). Principal investigators and co-investigators from the sites are as follows: the University of California, Berkeley/San Francisco: Stephen P. Hinshaw, Ph.D. (Berkeley), Glen R. Elliott, Ph.D., M.D. (San Francisco); Duke University: Karen C. Wells, Ph.D., Jeffery N. Epstein, Ph.D. (currently at Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center), Desiree W. Murray, Ph.D. Previous Duke contributors to early phases: C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. (former principal investigator), John March, M.D., M.P.H.; University of California, Irvine: James Swanson, Ph.D., Timothy Wigal, Ph.D. Previous contributor from UCLA to the early phases: Dennis P. Cantwell, M.D. (deceased); New York University: Howard B. Abikoff, Ph.D.; Montreal Children’s Hospital/ McGill University: Lily Hechtman, M.D.; New York State Psychiatric Institute/Columbia University/Mount Sinai Medical Center: Laurence L. Greenhill, M.D. (Columbia), Jeffrey H. Newcorn, M.D. (Mount Sinai School of Medicine); University of Pittsburgh: Brooke Molina, Ph.D., Betsy Hoza, Ph.D. (currently at University of Vermont), William E. Pelham, Ph.D. (principal investigator for early phases, currently at Florida International University). Follow-up phase statistical collaborators: Robert D. Gibbons, Ph.D. (University of Illinois, Chicago), Sue Marcus, Ph.D. (Mt. Sinai College of Medicine), Kwan Hur, Ph.D. (University of Illinois, Chicago). Original study statistical and design consultant: Helena C. Kraemer, Ph.D. (Stanford University). Collaborator from the Office of Special Education Programs/U.S. Department of Education: Thomas Hanley, Ed.D. Collaborator from the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention/U.S. Department of Justice: Karen Stern, Ph.D.

Clinical trials number: NCT00000388, Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (MTA) (https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT00000388).

Dr. Sibley receives book royalties from Guilford Press. Dr. Rohde has served on the speakers’ bureaus and/or acted as a consultant for Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Medice, Novartis, and Shire; he receives authorship royalties from Oxford University Press and ArtMed; the ADHD and Pediatric Bipolar Disorder Outpatient Programs chaired by him received unrestricted educational and research support from Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Novartis, and Shire; and he has received travel grants from Shire to attend the 2015 World Federation of ADHD Annual Meeting and from Novartis to attend the 2016 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Annual Meeting. Dr. Swanson has received research support, advisory board membership fees, speaker’s bureau membership fees, and/or consulting fees from Alza, Celgene, Celltech, Cephalon, CIBA, Gliatech, Janssen, Lilly, McNeil, Novartis, Shire, Richwood, Watson, and UCB, as well as travel funds and an honorarium for meeting with NLS Pharma. Dr. Hechtman has received research support from, served on advisory boards for, and served as a speaker for Ely Lilly, IronShore, Ortho Janssen, Purdue, and Shire. Dr. Mitchell has received royalties from New Harbinger Press. Dr. Arnold has received research funding from Curemark, Forest, Lilly, Neuropharm, Novartis, Noven, Roche, Shire, Supernus, and YoungLiving (as well as NIH and Autism Speaks); served as a consultant for Gowlings, Neuropharm, Organon, Pfizer, Shire, Sigma Tau, Tris Pharma, and Waypoint; served on advisory boards for Arbor, Ironshore, Novartis, Noven, Otsuka, Pfizer, Roche, Seaside Therapeutics, Shire, and Sigma Tau; and received travel support from Noven. All other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Olfson M, Blanco C, Wang S, et al.: National trends in the mental health care of children, adolescents, and adults by office-based physicians. JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71:81–90Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 McGough JJ: Treatment controversies in adult ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 2016; 173:960–966LinkGoogle Scholar

3 Benson K, Flory K, Humphreys KL, et al.: Misuse of stimulant medication among college students: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2015; 18:50–76Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Agnew-Blais JC, Polanczyk GV, Danese A, et al.: Evaluation of the persistence, remission, and emergence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:713–720Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Caye A, Rocha TB, Anselmi L, et al.: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: evidence from a birth cohort supporting a late-onset syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:705–712Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Moffitt TE, Houts R, Asherson P, et al.: Is adult ADHD a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from a four-decade longitudinal cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2015; 172:967–977LinkGoogle Scholar

7 Riglin L, Collishaw S, Thapar AK, et al.: Association of genetic risk variants with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories in the general population. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:1285–1292Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Li D, Sham PC, Owen MJ, et al.: Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Hum Mol Genet 2006; 15:2276–2284Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Nigg JT: Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychol Bull 2001; 127:571–598Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Zang YF, He Y, Zhu CZ, et al.: Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Dev 2007; 29:83–91Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Faraone SV, Biederman J: Can attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder onset occur in adulthood? JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:655–656Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE, et al.: Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:1313–1323Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Castellanos FX: Is adult-onset ADHD a distinct entity? Am J Psychiatry 2015; 172:929–931LinkGoogle Scholar

14 Solanto MV: Child vs adult onset of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 2017; 74:421Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 The MTA Cooperative Group: A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:1073–1086Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Jensen PS, Arnold LE, Swanson JM, et al.: 3-year follow-up of the NIMH MTA Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007; 46:989–1002Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Molina BS, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, et al.: The MTA at 8 years: prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009; 48:484–500Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Molina BSG, et al. Young adult outcomes in the follow-up of the Multimodal Treatment Study of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: symptom persistence, source discrepancy, and height suppression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017; 58:663–678Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, et al.: NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:28–38Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Swanson JM: School-Based Assessments and Interventions for ADD Students. Irvine, Calif, KC Publications, 1992Google Scholar

21 Sibley MH, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, et al.: Defining ADHD symptom persistence in adulthood: optimizing sensitivity and specificity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017; 58:655–662Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Conners CK, Erhardt D, Sparrow E: Conner’s Adult ADHD Rating Scales: CAARS. Toronto, MHS, 1999Google Scholar

23 Swanson JM, Kraemer HC, Hinshaw SP, et al.: Clinical relevance of the primary findings of the MTA: success rates based on severity of ADHD and ODD symptoms at the end of treatment. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40:168–179Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Bird HR, Shaffer D, Fisher P, et al.: The Columbia Impairment Scale (CIS): pilot findings on a measure of global impairment for children and adolescents. Int J Meth Psy Res 1993; 3:167–176Google Scholar

25 Bird HR, Andrews H, Schwab-Stone M, et al.: Global measures of impairment for epidemiologic and clinical use with children and adolescents. Int J Meth Psy Res 1997; 6:295–307CrossrefGoogle Scholar

26 Fabiano GA, Pelham WE Jr, Waschbusch DA, et al.: A practical measure of impairment: psychometric properties of the impairment rating scale in samples of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and two school-based samples. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2006; 35:369–385Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Molina BS, Pelham WE Jr: Childhood predictors of adolescent substance use in a longitudinal study of children with ADHD. J Abnorm Psychol 2003; 112:497–507Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Molina BS, Hinshaw SP, Eugene Arnold L, et al.: Adolescent substance use in the Multimodal Treatment Study of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (MTA) as a function of childhood ADHD, random assignment to childhood treatments, and subsequent medication. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013; 52:250–263Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Molina BSG, Sibley MH: The Case for including informant reports in the assessment of adulthood ADHD. ADHD Rep 2014; 22:1–7CrossrefGoogle Scholar

30 Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, et al.: The persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2002; 111:279–289Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Evans SW, Allen J, Moore S, et al.: Measuring symptoms and functioning of youth with ADHD in middle schools. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2005; 33:695–706Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Fischer M: ADHD in Adults: What the Science Says. New York, Guilford, 2008Google Scholar

33 Murphy K, Gordon M, Barkley RA: To what extent are ADHD symptoms common? A reanalysis of standardization data from a DSM-IV checklist. ADHD Rep 2000; 8:1–5Google Scholar

34 Sibley MH, Mitchell JT, Becker SP: Method of adult diagnosis influences estimated persistence of childhood ADHD: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Lancet Psychiatry 2016; 3:1157–1165Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 5th ed. Washington DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013CrossrefGoogle Scholar

36 Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S: Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006; 47:296–312Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA: The adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1124:111–126Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Moffitt TE, Caspi A: Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Dev Psychopathol 2001; 13:355–375Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Chandra S, Biederman J, Faraone SV: Assessing the validity of the age at onset criterion for diagnosing ADHD in DSM-5. J Atten Disord 2016; 1087054716629717CrossrefGoogle Scholar

40 McAllister TW, Zafonte R, Jain S, et al.: Randomized placebo-controlled trial of methylphenidate or galantamine for persistent emotional and cognitive symptoms associated with PTSD and/or traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016; 41:1191–1198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar