Skip to main content

Theoretical and Empirical Relationships between Personality and Intelligence

  • Chapter
International Handbook of Personality and Intelligence

Part of the book series: Perspectives on Individual Differences ((PIDF))

Abstract

What theoretical relationships have been postulated between intelligence and personality? The short answer is that there are very few in the mainstream of Western personality research. Even books titled Intelligence and Personality (Heim, 1970) turn out to be more about intelligence than personality. Despite the separation that generally exists between psychologists working in these areas, there are some cases (e.g., Baron, 1982) where there is clearly a sufficient communality of interests for the different approaches to overlap. This is evident for areas of intellectual functioning like creativity or cognitive style (see Brody, 1972), but the prevailing tendency is to treat intelligence and personality as independent.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and development: A cognitive theory. Oxford, England: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M., Martin, M., and Crossland, G. (1989). Happiness as a function of social encounters. In J. P. Forgas and M. J. Innes (Eds.), Recent advances in social psychology: An international perspective (pp. 189–193 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (1982). Personality and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, P. T., Daum, I., and Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Sensory nerve conduction and intelligence: A methodological study. Journal of Psychophysiology, 4, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartussek, D., Diedrich, O., Naumann, E., and Collett, W. (1993). Introversion-extraversion and event-related potential (ERP): A test of J. A. Gray’s theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 565–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, T., and Eysenck, H. J. (1993). String length, attention and intelligence: Focussed attention reverses the string length-IQ relationship. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 363–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bazylevich, T. F. (1974). The syndrome of strength of the regulative brain system. Nauka, 8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, G. K., Seashore, H. G., and Wesman, A. G. (1989). Differential aptitude tests. Orlando, FL: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., and Simon, T. (1905). Application des methodes nouvelles au diagnostic du niveau intellectual chez des enfants normal et anormaux d’hospice et d’ecole primiaire. L’Annee Psychologique, I1, 245–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brannigan, G. G., Ash, T., and Margolis, H. (1980). Impulsivity-reflectivity and children’s intellectual performance. Journal of Personality Assessment, 44, 41–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J. (1983a). A comment on Paisey and Mangan’s neo-Pavlovian temperament theory and the biological bases of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 229–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J. (1983b). A model of extraversion. Australian Journal of Psychology, 35, 349–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J. (1990a). Personality factors in stress and anxiety. In C. D. Spielberger, R. Diaz-Guerrero, and and J. Strelau (Eds.), Cross-cultural anxiety (Vol. 4 ). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J. (1990b). Psychological and neurophysiological factors in stimulus-response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor and T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 241–260 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J. (1991). Personality and generalization as a source of stress. In I. Sarason, C. D. Spielberger, J. Strelau, and J. Brebner (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 14 ). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J., and Cooper, C. (1974). The effect of a low rate of regular signals upon the reaction times of introverts and extraverts. Journal of Research in Personality, 8, 263–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J., and Cooper, C. (1978). Stimulus or response-induced excitation: A comparison of the behavior of introverts and extraverts. Journal of Research in Personality, 12, 306–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J., and Cooper, C. (1986). Personality factors and inspection time. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 709–714.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J., Donaldson, J., Kirby, N., and Ward, L. (in press). Happiness measures, psychoticism and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, N. (1972). Personality: Research and theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, N. (1982). Intelligence ( 2nd ed. ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, N., and Kihlstrom, J. E (1987). Personality and social intelligence. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlier, M. (1979). An attempt to validate Strelau’s questionnaire. Paper presented at the International Conference on Temperament, Need for Stimulation, and Activity, Warsaw.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., and Tatsuoka, M. M. (1970). Handbook for the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, J. F. (1986). Evoked potential augmenting-reducing: A weak link in the biology-personality chain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 746–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, D. W. (1972). Studies of individual differences at the Applied Psychology Unit. In V. D. Nebylitsyn and J. A. Gray (Eds.), Biological bases of individual behavior (pp. 269–290 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Toledo, L., Evers, S., Hoeppner, T. J., and Morrell, F. (1991). A “stress” test for memory dysfunction: Electrophysiological manifestations of early Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 48, 605–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1933). How we think: A restatement of the relation of reflective thinking to the educative process. Boston: Heath.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ditraglia, G. M., and Polich, J. (1991). P300 and introverted/ extraverted personality types. Psychophysiology, 28, 177–184. Edwards, E. (1964). Information transmission: An introductory guide to the application of the theory of information to the human sciences. London: Chapman and Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G., and Nettelbeck, T. (1993). Notes and shorter communication. Inspection time: A flash mask to reduce apparent movement effects. Personality and Individual Differences, 15 (1): 91–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: C. C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1971). Relation between intelligence and personality. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 32, 637–638.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1982). A model for intelligence. London: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1987). Speed of information processing, reaction time, and the theory of intelligence. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1988). The concept of “intelligence”: Useful or useless? Intelligence, 12, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J., and Barrett, P. (1985). Psychophysiology and the measurement of intelligence. In C. R. Reynolds and V. L. Wilson (Eds.), Methodological and statistical advances in the study of individual differences. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J., and Cookson, D. (1969). Personality in primary schoolchildren. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 39, 109–122.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, S. B. G., Pearson, P. R., Easting, G., and Allsopp, J. B. (1985). Age norms for Impulsiveness, Venturesomeness, and Empathy in adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 613–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrington, D. P. (1991). Psychological contributions to the explanation of offending. Issues in Criminological and Legal Psychology, 1, 7–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, J. R. (1987). The ontology of intelligence. In H. Forge (Ed.), Measurement, realism and objectivity. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, S. S., and O’Brien, S. (1965). Duplication of evoked potential waveform by curve of probability of firing a single cell. Science, 147, 888–890.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • French, J. W., Exstrom, R. B., and Price, L. A. (1963). Kit of references for cognitive factors. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furnham, A. (1992). Personality and learning style: A study of three instruments. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 429–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gale, A. (1981). EEG studies of extraversion-introversion. In R. Lynn (Ed.), Dimensions of personality: Papers in honour of H. J. Eysenck. Oxford, England: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gale, A. (1984). O’Gorman versus Gale: A reply. Biological Psychology, 19, 129–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. (1972). The psychophysiological nature of introversion-extraversion: A modification of Eysenck’s theory. In V. D. Nebylitsyn and J. A. Gray (Eds.), Biological bases of individual behavior (pp. 151–169 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1959). Three faces of intellect. American Psychologist, 17, 459–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gupta, U. (1988). Effects of impulsivity and caffeine on human cognitive performance. Pharmacopsychologeia, 1, 33–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haler, R. J., Robinson, D. L., Braden, W., and Williams, D. (1983). Electrical potentials of the cerebral cortex and psychometric intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 591–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hakstian, A. R., and Cattell, R. B. (1982). Manual for the comprehensive ability battery. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heim, A. (1970). Intelligence and personality. Harmondsworth, England: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrickson, A. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence: II. Measurement. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 151–196). New York: Springer-Verlag. Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence: I. Theory. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 197–228 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrickson, D. E., and Hendrickson, A. E. (1980). The biological basis for individual differences in intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 3–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. L. (1985). The self-directed search. Hawthorn, Victoria: Australian Council for Educational Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzman, P. S. (1954). The relation of assimilation tendencies in visual, auditory, and kinesthetic time-error to cognitive attitudes of leveling and sharpening. Journal of Personality, 22, 375–394.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Honey, P., and Mumford, A. (1982). The manual of learning styles. Maidenhead, England: Honey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L. (1985). Remodelling old models of intelligence. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 462–503 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1982). Reaction time and psychometric g. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1987). Individual differences in the Hick paradigm. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R., and Munro, E. (1979). Reaction time, movement time and intelligence. Intelligence, 3, 121–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, L. C. (1957). Generality of speed and confidence in judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 264–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jubel, J. (1991). Relationships between psychometric intelligence and information processing speed indices. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 11, 73–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, W., and Wallach, M. A. (1964). Risk taking: A study in cognition and personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kipnis, D. (1971). Character structure and impulsiveness. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, P. M., Cowles, M. P., and Lafreniere, K. (1987). Relationships between psychometric and experimental measures of arousability. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 225–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, D. (1976). Learning Style Inventory: Technical Manual. Boston, MA: McBer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozcielak, R. (1979). The role of the nervous system traits in inventive creativity. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 10, 225–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kranzler, J. H., and Jensen, A. R. (1989). Inspection time and intelligence: A meta analysis. Intelligence, 13, 329–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrl, S., and Fischer, B. (1990). A basic information psychological parameter (BIP) for the reconstruction of concepts of intelligence. European Journal of Personality, 4, 259–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lolas, F., Camposano, S., and Etcheberrigaray, R. (1989). Augmenting/reducing and personality: A psychometric and evoked potential study in a Chilean sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1173–1176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lolas, F., Etcheberrigaray, R., Elqueta, D., and Camposano, S. (1989). Visual evoked potential reducing: A vertex feature of late components. Research, Communications in Psychology, Psychiatry and Behavior, 14, 173–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukas, J. H. (1987). Visual evoked potential augmenting-reducing and personality: The vertex augmenter is a sensation seeker. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 385–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, R., and Eysenck, H. J. (1961). Tolerance for pain, extraversion and neuroticism. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 12, 161–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1986). The biology of intelligence? British Journal of Psychology, 77, 1–18.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., and Tara, P. (1986). Attention bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mangan, G. L. (1982). The biology of human conduct. Sydney: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattarazzo, J. D. (1992). Psychological testing and assessment in the 21st century. American Psychologist, 47, 1007–1018.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G. (1987). Personality and multidimensional arousal: A study of two dimensions of extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 9–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., and Amelang, M. (1993). Extraversion, arousal theory and performance: A study of individual differences in the EEG. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 347–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettelbeck, T. (1987). Inspection time and intelligence. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence (pp. 295–346 ). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettelbeck, T., and Rabbitt, P. (1992). Aging, cognitive performance, and mental speed. Intelligence, 16, 189–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neubauer, A. C., Bauer, C., and Holler, G. (1992). Intelligence, attention, motivation and speed-accuracy trade-off in the Hick paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1325–1332.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Gorman, J. G. (1977). Individual differences in habituation of human physiological responses: A review of theory, method, and findings in the study of personality correlates in non-clinical populations. Biological Psychology, 5, 257–318.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Gorman, J. G., and Lloyd, J. E. M. (1987). Extraversion, impulsiveness, and EEG alpha activity. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 169–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Gorman, J. G., and Mallise, L. R. (1984). Extraversion and the EEG: 2. A test of Gale’s hypothesis. Biological Psychology, 19, 113–127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pamula, D. (1993). Environmental attitudes, knowledge, and behavior, and their relationship with demographic variables and personality characteristics. Unpublished honors thesis, University of Adelaide, South Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrie, A. (1967). Individuality in pain and suffering. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, T. F. (1958). The measurement and correlates of category width as a cognitive variable. Journal of Personality, 26, 532–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polich, J. (1987). Task difficulty, probability, and interstimulus interval as determinants of P300 from auditory stimuli. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 68, 311–320.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polich, J., Ladish, C., and Bloom, F. E. (1990). P300 assessment of early Alzheimer’s disease. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 77, 179–189.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ragot, R. (1984). Perceptual and motor space representation: An event-related potential study. Psychophysiology, 21, 159–170.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A., and Venables, P. H. (1988). Enhanced P3 evoked potentials and longer P3 recovery times in psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 25, 30–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Revelle, W., Humphreys, M. S., Simon, L., and Gilliland, K. (1980). The interactive effect of personality, time of day, and caffeine: A test of the arousal model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. L. (1982a). Properties of the diffuse thalamocortical system and human personality: A direct test of the Pavlovian/Eysenckian theory. Personality and Individual Dif-ferences, 3, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. L. (1982b). Properties of the diffuse thalamocortical system, human intelligence, and differentiated vs. integrated modes of learning. Personality and Individual Differences, 3, 393–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. L. (1983). The diffuse thalamocortical system and Pavlovian/Eysenckian theory: A response to criticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 535–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. L. (1985). How personality relates to intelligence test performance: Implications for a theory of intelligence, ageing research and personality assessment. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 203–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. L. (1986). The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and personality assessment: Towards a biologically based theory of intelligence and cognition. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 153–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. L. (1989). The neurophysiological bases of high IQ. International Journal of Neuroscience, 46, 209–234.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, T. N., and Zahn, T. P. (1985). Psychoticism and arousal: Possible evidence for a linkage of P and psychopathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 47–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rusalov, V. M. (1989). Object-related and communicative aspects of human temperament: A new questionnaire of the structure of temperament. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 817–827

    Google Scholar 

  • Saklofske, D. H. (1985). The relationship between Eysenck’s major personality dimensions and simultaneous and sequential processing in children. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 429–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saklofske, D. H., and Kostura, D. D. (1990). Extraversion-introversion and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 547–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shagass, C., Roemer, R. A., Straumanis, J. J., and Josiassen, R. C. (1981). Intelligence as a factor in evoked potential studies of psychopathology: I. Comparison of low and high IQ subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 16, 1007–1030.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stelmack, R. M., Achorn, E., and Michaud, A. (1977). Extraversion and individual differences in auditory evoked response. Psychophysiology, 14, 368–374.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stelmack, R. M., Houlihan, M., and McGarry-Roberts, P. A. (1993). Personality, reaction time, and event-related potentials. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 399–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stelmack, R. M., and Plouffe, L. (1983). Introversion-extraversion: the Bell-Magendie law revisited. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 421–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenberg, G. (1992). Personality and the EEG: Arousal and emotional arousability. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1097–1113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenberg, G., Rosen, I., and Risberg, J. (1988). Personality and augmenting/reducing in visual and auditory evoked potentials. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 571–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenberg, G., Rosen, I., and Risberg, J. (1990). Attention and personality in augmenting/reducing of visual evoked potentials. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 1243–1254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stott, D. H. (1985). Learning style or “intelligence”? School Psychology International, 6, 167–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stough, C., Bates, T., Colrain, I., Mangan, G. L., and O. (in press). Apparent motion, backward masking, inspection time and IQ. Personality and Individual Differences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stough, C., Mangan, G. L., Pellett, O. L., and Bates, T. C. (1993). Jensen’s reaction time-intelligence paradigm: Effects of extraversion and strategy. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stough, C., and Nettelbeck, T. (1989). Inspection time and IQ [Letter]. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 2, 341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stough, C. Nettelbeck, T., and Cooper, C. (1990). Evoked brain potentials, string length and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 401–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stough, C., Nettelbeck, T., and Cooper, C. J. (1993). The relationship between elementary information processing, personality and intelligence. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strelau, J. (1970). Nervous system type and extraversion-introversion: A comparison of Eysenck’s theory with Pavlov’s typology. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 1, 17–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strelau, J. (1980). Regulatory functions of temperament. Wroclaw, Poland: Ossolineum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strelau, J. (1983). Temperament-Personality-Activity. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strelau, J. (1987). Personality dimensions based on arousal theories: Search for integration. In J. Strelau and H. J. Eysenck (Eds.), Personality dimensions and arousal (pp. 269–286 ). London: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teplov, B. M. (1956). Problems in the study of general types of higher nervous activity in man and animals. In B. M. Teplov (Ed.), Typological features of higher nervous activity in man (Vol. 1 ). Moscow: Academy of Pedagogical Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P. A. (1990). The use of biological measures to estimate behavioral intelligence. Educational Psychologist, 25, 293–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, D., Nettelbeck, T., and Wilson, R. J. (1972). Perceptual indices of performance: the measurement of “inspection time” in the visual system. Perception, 1, 263–295.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, D., and Smith, P. (1985). The rationale for the inspection time index. Paper presented at meeting of the International Society for the Study of Individual Differences, Saint Feliu de Guixols, Catalonia, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, E, Kruger, J., Schalt, E., Schnobel, R., and Hassling, L. (1987). No consistent relationships between oscillations and latencies of visual and auditory evoked potentials and measures of mental performance. Human Neurobiology, 6, 173–182.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., James, M., and Maciejewski, C. (1986). The WAIS as a lateralizing and localizing diagnostic instrument: A study of 656 patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Neuro-psychology, 24, 223–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, V. (1989). From short-term memory capacity toward the EEG resonance code. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 501–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welford, A. T. (ed.). (1980). Reaction times. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welford, A. T. (1986). Longstreth versus Jensen and Vernon on reaction time and IQ: An outsider’s view. Intelligence, 10, 193–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whetton, D., and Cameron, K. (1984). Development Management Skills. London: Scott, Foreman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whipple, S. C., Parker, E. S., and Noble, E. P. (1988). An atypical neurocognitive profile in alcoholic fathers and their sons. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 49, 240–244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, P. O. (1973). Individual differences in speed, accuracy and persistence: A mathematical model for problem solving. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), The measurement of intelligence. Lancaster, England: Medical and Technical Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Y., Caryl, P. G., and Deary, I. J. (1989). Evoked potentials, inspection time and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1079–1094.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1984). Sensation seeking: A comparative approach to a human trait. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 413–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1990). The psychophysiology of sensation seeking. Journal of Personality, 58, 313–345.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1995 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Brebner, J., Stough, C. (1995). Theoretical and Empirical Relationships between Personality and Intelligence. In: Saklofske, D.H., Zeidner, M. (eds) International Handbook of Personality and Intelligence. Perspectives on Individual Differences. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-5571-8_16

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-5571-8_16

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4419-3239-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-5571-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics