Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 19 December 2014
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognition across the psychiatric disorder spectrum: From mental health to clinical diagnosis View all 12 articles

An Overview of the Association between Schizotypy and Dopamine

  • 1Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany

Schizotypy refers to a constellation of personality traits that are believed to mirror the subclinical expression of schizophrenia in the general population. Evidence from pharmacological studies indicates that dopamine (DA) is involved in the etiology of schizophrenia. Based on the assumption of a continuum between schizophrenia and schizotypy, researchers have begun investigating the association between DA and schizotypy using a wide range of methods. In this article, we review published studies on this association from the following areas of work: (1) experimental investigations of the interactive effects of dopaminergic challenges and schizotypy on cognition, motor control, and behavior (2), dopaminergically supported cognitive functions (3), studies of associations between schizotypy and polymorphisms in genes involved in dopaminergic neurotransmission, and (4) molecular imaging studies of the association between schizotypy and markers of the DA system. Together, data from these lines of evidence suggest that DA is important to the expression and experience of schizotypy and associated behavioral biases. An important observation is that the experimental designs, methods, and manipulations used in this research are highly heterogeneous. Future studies are required to replicate individual observations, to enlighten the link between DA and different schizotypy dimensions (positive, negative, cognitive disorganization), and to guide the search for solid DA-sensitive behavioral markers. Such studies are important in order to clarify inconsistencies between studies. More work is also needed to identify differences between dopaminergic alterations in schizotypy compared to the dysfunctions observed in schizophrenia.

Introduction

Schizotypy, first coined by Rado (1), is a set of personality traits thought to mirror the subclinical expression of schizophrenia in the general population. Schizotypy encompasses behaviors, cognitions, and emotions resembling the signs and symptoms of schizophrenia. It is typically assessed in the general population using psychometric self-report questionnaires including, among others, the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) (2), the Oxford–Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE) (3), the Chapman scales (4), and the Rust Inventory of Schizotypal Cognitions (RISC) (5). Factor analytic studies have shown that schizotypy consists broadly of three subdimensions (3, 6). The positive schizotypy dimension describes perceptual aberrations resembling the hallucinations of schizophrenia as well as unusual views and ideas resembling the delusions of schizophrenia. The negative schizotypy dimension describes a loss of normal emotional, physical, and social functions such as the experience of pleasure or interest in social contacts. Finally, the disorganized dimension encompasses eccentric behavior and odd speech. Overall, the symptomatic phenomenology of schizotypy thus resembles the factor structure of symptoms reported in schizophrenia (7).

The literature tends to draw upon two scientific approaches toward the study of schizotypy, i.e., the quasi-dimensional and fully dimensional approaches. The quasi-dimensional, psychiatrically oriented approach, proposed by Meehl (8, 9), regards schizotypy as the subclinical expression of the symptoms of schizophrenia (10, 11). In that approach, schizotypal individuals are hypothesized to carry the genetic risk for schizophrenia. Evidence for this approach stems from some (12), but not all (13), taxometric studies. The fully dimensional approach (14), championed by Eysenck and Claridge (13, 1518), regards schizotypy as a personality trait which is continuously distributed. It is assumed that schizotypal symptoms in the healthy population are qualitatively similar though quantitatively milder than those observed in schizophrenia. Evidence for the fully dimensional approach stems from taxometric studies that consider positive skewness of sample distribution (13). The high prevalence of psychotic-like experiences in the general population is also in accordance with the fully dimensional approach (19, 20). Importantly, both approaches emphasize the observation of variation in schizotypal features in the population. More work is, however, needed on the exact nature of the distribution and the boundary between non-clinical schizotypy and clinical schizophrenia (12, 13, 21, 22).

Irrespective of these open psychometric questions, biopsychological research on schizotypy is also based on the continuum assumption. There is considerable evidence that behavioral and brain correlates of schizophrenia are also related to schizotypy [for review, see, e.g., Ref. (11, 23, 24)]. By and large, the focus of these schizotypy studies has been on behavioral, cognitive, brain structural, and brain functional measures. Neuronal information transfer is, however, only possible with functional neurochemical transmissions at synapses. Therefore, neurochemical studies involving pharmacological challenges or molecular imaging methods are of utmost importance for our understanding of schizotypy. Most prominent so far are studies that link schizotypy to dopamine (DA). Comparable to above mentioned study domains, this link originates in the continuum assumption because schizophrenia has been linked to DA (25, 26), schizotypy might be associated with DA as well. In the following, we will explore this possible link. We will describe a selection of relevant psychopharmacological studies in patients with schizophrenia before referring to schizotypy studies that test the possible link to DA. First, however, we briefly introduce the structure and function of the DA system.

At this point, it should be mentioned that the schizophrenia spectrum also of course includes individuals with schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) and those at increased genetic or clinical risk for the illness. However, in the interest of focus, we restrict this overview to psychometrically identified schizotypy. Of course, it should be acknowledged that some, but not all, individuals with high levels of schizotypy also qualify for a diagnosis of SPD. For example, Raine (2) observed SPD in 55% of participants with SPQ scores in the top 10% of the distribution.

The Structure and Function of Human Dopamine Systems

Neurotransmitters exert different actions throughout the brain; the type of action depends on the neurotransmitter, action site, and/or neuronal circuit (27). For instance, the amino acids GABA (inhibitory) and glutamate (excitatory) are found throughout the brain. Acetylcholine and monoamines (DA, norepinephrine, serotonin), on the other hand, are organized in systems. Systems imply that cell bodies producing these substances are located subcortically in anatomically circumscribed regions (27). In the case of DA, the nigrostriatal (important to motor control), mesolimbic (important to reward and motivation), and mesocortical (important to prefrontal functions) pathways have been distinguished on the basis of where their cell bodies are located and where their axons project. Moreover, two major DA receptor families have been identified, viz. the D1-like (D1 and D5) and D2-like (D2, D3, D4) receptors that are widely distributed throughout the brain. Because of the importance of the DA system to various mental health conditions, its complexity and functioning remain intensively investigated (28).

Of course, complex mental health disorders cannot be explained by single changes to complex neurotransmitter systems. Instead, subsystems are likely contributors to different psychopathologies. DA has indeed been associated with diverse neurological and psychiatric conditions such as Parkinson’s disease, schizophrenia, addiction, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [e.g., Ref. (29, 30)]. Also, neurotransmitter systems are not modular, i.e., they do not exert their actions in isolation. A given neurotransmitter system is in constant interaction with other systems. In schizophrenia, interactions between the DA and glutamate systems are intensively debated (31). Accordingly, pharmacological compounds can enhance DA release while also enhancing the release of the other two monoamines (norepinephrine, serotonin). The latter release might happen directly or indirectly through metabolism of DA into norepinephrine and the latter into serotonin (32, 33). This overall complexity in neurotransmitter systems highlights the fact that individual psychopharmacological studies have inherent limitations in investigating the link between schizotypy and DA. As exemplified in the following sections, we depend on findings from a multitude of studies using different but complementary approaches.

Dopaminergic Effects in Patients with Schizophrenia

Neuroleptics were first discovered in the early 1950s (34). Since their discovery, it has been shown that these DA antagonists ameliorate acute psychotic symptoms [e.g., Ref. (35, 36)]. It is still accepted that DA is key in the treatment and pathophysiology of schizophrenia (3739). For example, patients with schizophrenia profit from DA antagonistic treatment (38) and show symptom worsening after DA agonistic treatment (40, 41). In healthy populations, DA agonists can trigger psychotic or psychosis-like symptoms (42, 43). Such findings led to the suggestion that a hyperactive DA system results in acute psychotic symptoms (25, 38). Davis et al. (25) further elaborated on this suggestion by proposing DA abnormalities to depend on the brain region, i.e., D1 receptors being predominantly found cortically and D2 receptors subcortically. They specified that frontal hypodopaminergia would result in striatal hyperdopaminergia. Regarding symptoms, they related negative symptoms to frontal hypodopaminergia and positive symptoms to striatal hyperdopaminergia.

Howes and Kapur (44) refined this previous DA theory by suggesting that multiple “hits” (e.g., social, environmental, and cultural stressors) result in DA dysregulation at the pre-synaptic level. Such DA dysregulation is hypothesized to lead to changes in how events are appraised, potentially because events become overly salient (i.e., attract increased allocation of meaning). According to these authors, an alteration in pre-synaptic DA is the final common pathway to psychosis and psychosis-proneness (45). Yet, other evidence points to the role of post-synaptic processes (46). Very recently, Seeman (39) outlined how dynamic changes of post-synaptic D2 receptors might contribute to symptoms experienced in schizophrenia. Mainly based on animal studies, Seeman reports on D2 receptors that can either take a high affinity state for DA (D2High) or a low affinity state for DA (D2Low). Reviewing the literature, he suggests that psychosis is associated with the more active and normally less common D2High state.

Overall, despite various suggestions on the involvement of alternative neurotransmitter systems in psychosis and, by inference, schizophrenia (31, 47), recent notions highlight that DA is the likely final common pathway to psychosis. The precise mechanisms and sites of action remain inconclusive, and continued research reveals ever more complex synaptic dynamics such as the ones described for the D2High and D2Low affinity states. It remains to be clarified whether such alterations of the DA system are specific to overt clinical psychosis or are also evident, though less pronounced, along the schizophrenia spectrum including healthy schizotypy. Howes and Kapur (44) previously reviewed a number of studies showing an involvement of DA in the prodromal state, in ultra-high risk populations, in relatives of schizophrenia patients and in schizotypy. Because these populations lie on the psychosis continuum, part of these and additional studies will be discussed in more detail.

In an 18F-dopa positron emission tomography (PET) study, Howes et al. (48) observed that patients with prodromal psychotic symptoms showed enhanced striatal 18F-dopa uptake, intermediate to that of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. This enhanced uptake correlated positively with prodromal symptomatology and neuropsychological impairments [but see in Ref. (49)]. In a 3-year follow-up study, Howes et al. (50) observed that greater striatal DA synthesis capacity was observed in a psychosis transition group as compared to a group that did not transit into psychosis. Moreover, this capacity in the transition group correlated positively with symptom severity. In another study, elevated pre-synaptic striatal uptake was comparable between patients with SPD and remitted patients with schizophrenia (51). It was also elevated for individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis, i.e., those who show impaired socio-occupational functions and attenuated psychotic symptoms (e.g., perceptual abnormalities, paranoia) (52). An increase of striatal pre-synaptic DA synthesis has also been observed in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia (53). Relatives additionally showed an altered stress response, demonstrated by increased levels of plasma DA metabolites when compared to healthy controls (54). This increase was associated with psychotic-like symptoms subsequent to daily stressors (experience sampling methodology) (55). In yet another study, psychotic patients and unaffected relatives released significantly more DA in striatal regions subsequent to the inhalation of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (the main psychoactive ingredient of cannabis) than healthy controls (56). In sum, these studies show that DA abnormalities are not restricted to individuals with a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia, but are also evident in the extended phenotype. Findings specific to schizotypy are presented further below.

Methods in the Study of Dopamine and Its Relation to Schizotypy and Cognition

Various methods are available to measure aspects of the DA system in healthy humans. The most promising to study the link between schizotypy and DA include (1) experimental pharmacological challenges of the DA system, (2) neurobiologically informed, DA-sensitive cognitive and behavioral measures, (3) molecular studies of genes involved in DA neurotransmission, and (4) molecular neuroimaging of the DA system. Here, we briefly outline these methods. In the next section, we discuss how they have been used in work on schizotypy.

The first methodological approach directly tests the link between schizotypy and DA. For instance, one could test DA agonistic and antagonistic treatment effects on the experience of schizotypy as well as related correlates in healthy populations. This approach corresponds to tests of medication effects in patients. In patients, however, pharmacological treatment consists of the daily administration of medication over many weeks, given that the clinical antipsychotic effects of such compounds take several weeks to emerge (57). When testing healthy populations, such long-term drug administration would be unethical and potentially dangerous. For instance, antipsychotic treatment causes severe side effects [e.g., Ref. (58) for a meta-analysis]. These and other (e.g., economic and clinical) factors explain why the investigation of long-term medication effects is common in clinical studies while single (or limited) short-term drug effects are frequently investigated in healthy volunteers.

Importantly, short-term dopaminergic drug applications directly modulate the DA system, thereby allowing the evaluation of dopaminergic effects on domains of cognition, emotion, motor control and their neural correlates. Such studies tend to take place in well-controlled laboratory settings using tight methodological controls, such as double-blind procedures and placebo conditions [e.g., Ref. (5961)]. Such paradigms inform on acute drug effects and their interaction with schizotypy. They also allow addressing clinical questions hard to implement in patients (4043). As outlined above, however, it should be borne in mind that the DA system does not work in isolation but instead interacts with other neurotransmitter systems; therefore, the specificity of any pharmacological effect on behavior and cognition remains to be characterized further.

The second methodological approach involves the assessment of DA-mediated cognitive and behavioral performance measures and their covariance with schizotypal personality traits. This approach is indirect in comparison to the first approach. Yet, the use of DA-mediated performance measures is popular. Various paradigms have been drawn upon (6267). Of importance, previous studies have shown that these performance measures are sensitive to modulations of the DA system. In particular, such measures are affected by dopaminergic drugs in humans [e.g., Ref. (61, 68, 69)], human mental health conditions in which the DA system is affected [e.g., Ref. (64, 65)] and by experimental dopaminergic interventions in animals [e.g., Ref. (64, 69)].

The third methodological approach involves molecular imaging methods. As indicated above, methods such as PET and single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) can provide in vivo data on markers of the DA system (such as DA synthesis or receptor and transporter availability) in the human brain (45). These markers can be quantified and related directly to schizotypy scores. As will be detailed below, PET and SPECT methods have recently been applied to study the DA system as a function of schizotypy.

The fourth methodological approach involves the exploration of the role of DA in schizotypy by studying statistical associations of schizotypy scores with variants in genes whose protein products are known to be involved in the DA system (70, 71). Such genes may include those coding for receptors, transporters or enzymes involved in DA synthesis, release, reuptake, degradation or any other aspect of DA transmission. The effects on DA transmission of such genetic variants, when known, allow drawing conclusions about differences in DA transmission in relation to individual differences in schizotypy.

Dopamine and Schizotypy: Empirical Evidence

A role of DA in schizophrenia and schizotypy has long been postulated (67, 7274). Empirical evidence on the nature of the association between DA and schizotypy has been scarce and the association has rarely been investigated directly (68, 75). However, studies have accumulated over the last 15 years. Here, we give an overview on the outcome of these studies, irrespective of which methodological approach has been applied. In case that DA was investigated indirectly (the second methodological approach mentioned above), we distinguish between studies that assessed more basic behavioral functions and those that assessed higher cognitive functions. Often these studies added a direct manipulation of the DA system using the first methodological approach outlined above. Therefore, we discuss these together with the purely behavioral data on simple or complex behavioral functions. In the case of molecular genetic and molecular imaging studies, many findings are based on associations with questionnaire scores. We discuss these studies separately.

Basic Behavioral Functions

Turning behavior is a behavioral marker of a relatively hyperactive DA system in one over the other hemisphere. Animal studies [Ref. (76) for overview] and a study testing patients with asymmetrical Parkinson’s disease (77) showed that whole-body turning occurs away from the hemisphere with the more active DA system. Acutely psychotic patients (65) and individuals with elevated positive schizotypal features (74) yielded a preference for left- over right-sided turns pointing to a relative hyperactive right-hemispheric DA system along the schizophrenia spectrum. In a study that directly tested the influence of DA on turning behavior, half of the participants received a placebo and the other half a non-specific DA agonist (levodopa) (78). Turning biases were investigated as a function of participants’ positive and negative schizotypal features. The authors found that elevated positive schizotypy associated with a left- over right-sided turning preference in the placebo group. In this same group, negative schizotypy associated with a right- over left-sided turning preference. In the levodopa group, these relationships were not strengthened, but actually reversed. The authors argued that a higher than normal DA availability might have balanced out pre-existing DA asymmetries. In another experiment on the same participants, schizotypy and levodopa treatments were unrelated to lateral turning biases in a computer-based object-rotation task (66).

Spontaneous eye blink rate (SBR) is indicative of cerebral DA activity. Eye blinks occur spontaneously, frequently, and mostly without awareness. They are primarily important for the health of the eye surface and clarity of vision. Decades ago, SBR has been suggested to be a sensitive marker of striatal DA activity (64). SBR has been associated with changes in concurrent cognitive processes, perceptual load, and level of arousal (79, 80). It is enhanced in schizophrenia, in particular in the unmedicated or drug-naïve state (64, 81, 82), and decreases with antipsychotic treatment (64, 82, 83). In line with Davis et al.’s (25) notion of a link between negative symptoms and hypodopaminergia on the one hand and positive symptoms and hyperdopaminergia on the other hand, negative symptoms were associated with a decreased SBR (83) and positive symptoms with an increased SBR (82, 83). Additionally, SBR increased with a DA agonistic treatment in healthy individuals (84, 85).

In a dopaminergic challenge study, half of the participants received a placebo while the others received an unspecific DA agonist (levodopa) (86). SBR did not differ between substance groups, but correlated positively with negative (but not positive) schizotypy scores after levodopa intake. The authors conjectured that this effect occurred because negative schizotypy is a priori related to hypodopaminergia. In another study, however, SBR correlated positively with participants’ psychoticism scores, a subdimension of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (87). Some authors argued that psychoticism forms part of the schizotypy spectrum (88). Other authors linked psychoticism to impulsiveness, lack of cooperation, rigidity, low superego control, low social sensitivity, low persistence, lack of anxiety, and lack of feelings of inferiority (89) – features associated with borderline and antisocial personality disorder rather than with the schizophrenia spectrum (90, 91). The psychoticism dimension seems indeed the least clear-cut dimension of the three Eysenckian dimensions (90, 91). Whatever the psychoticism dimension is measuring, the latter SBR finding suggests that psychoticism is probably more sensitive to the DA system subserving SBR than is schizotypy (or negative schizotypy) as such.

Stereotyped behavior is thought to be sensitive to DA. It is observable in schizophrenia (92, 93) and after amphetamine consumption in healthy individuals (69). In schizophrenia, stereotyped behavior occurs on the motor (e.g., walking backwards and forwards, repetitive jaw movements) (94) and higher cognitive level. For instance, perseverative errors in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test are elevated in schizophrenia [Ref. (95) for overview], SPD (96, 97) and as a function of both high positive and negative schizotypy (98, 99). Random number generation tasks show that patients with schizophrenia (100, 101), healthy participants after amphetamine administration (69), and healthy positive schizotypal participants (102, 103) yield stereotypical response biases when compared to respective controls. Finally, when rotating figures into advantageous target positions on a computer screen, left or right turns had to be applied dynamically to obtain maximal scores (66). Sticking stereotypically to one or the other direction would be disadvantageous. The latter study reported on two experiments with one being a between-subject levodopa placebo-controlled experiment. The authors observed that individuals with relatively high as compared to low positive schizotypy were behaving more stereotypically. Yet, in the levodopa group, high positive schizotypal individuals performed less stereotypically than the low positive schizotypal individuals. Comparable to the findings on turning behavior, results seem to suggest that a higher than normal DA availability in positive schizotypes balances out pre-existing DA-mediated behavioral abnormalities.

Prepulse inhibition (PPI) is another DA-sensitive measure that has been studied in relation to schizotypy. PPI is a cross-species phenomenon that refers to a reduction in the amplitude of the startle response to a strong sensory stimulus, the pulse, if this stimulus is preceded by 30–500 ms by a weak stimulus, the prepulse (104). The weak prestimulus is thought to evoke inhibitory mechanisms, which limit further stimulation until the processing of the prepulse has been completed, resulting in a reduced impact of the pulse. PPI is thought to reflect an early sensory gating process to avoid interference from simultaneous processing of several stimuli. It is, thus, a largely automatic measure of inhibitory function. Experimental studies in animals have shown that a cortico-striato-pallido-thalamic circuitry underlies PPI (105).

Patients with schizophrenia (106) and SPD (107) show reproducible reductions in the magnitude of PPI, which is thought to lead to sensory overstimulation causing cognitive and behavioral confusion. Also, PPI deficits are induced in numerous experimental models of schizophrenia through, e.g., isolation rearing (108), ketamine (109), sleep deprivation (110, 111), and phencyclidine (112). The robust and reliable PPI reduction in schizophrenia can be restored, at least partially, with antipsychotic treatment, with some advantage of atypical compounds over first-generation, typical antipsychotics (113119).

Importantly, reduced PPI has been observed in relation to elevated SPQ total, cognitive-perceptual and interpersonal scores (120), Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) psychosis-proneness scores (121) and Eysenckian psychoticism scores (122). Evidence from a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study of PPI points to reduced activation in the insula, putamen, thalamus, inferior parietal cortex, hippocampal gyrus, and fusiform gyrus in relation to higher levels of Eysenckian psychoticism scores (122). These BOLD patterns show some resemblance with the data obtained from samples of patients with schizophrenia (123). Of relevance to the present discussion, areas such as the putamen are rich in DA receptors, consistent with the hypothesis that PPI is at least in part dopaminergically mediated (124126).

A final basic DA-mediated paradigm is latent inhibition (LI) (62, 127, 128). Comparable to PPI, LI is a cross-species phenomenon, which is sensitive to pro- and antidopaminergic challenges (62). LI refers to the finding that non-reinforced pre-exposure to a stimulus, that is later to be conditioned, causes less efficient conditioning when the same stimulus is subsequently reinforced. This phenomenon has been observed in both humans and animals. LI has also been investigated in relation to schizophrenia and schizotypy. A recent review showed reduced LI in the acute phase of patients with schizophrenia and preserved LI in chronic schizophrenia (129). That review also found a modest but relatively consistent relationship between schizotypy and LI, particularly of positive schizotypy (130). In particular, higher levels of positive schizotypy seem to be associated with less LI in healthy participants. These studies, together, suggest an involvement of dopaminergic alterations in the LI deficits in schizophrenia and positive schizotypy.

Higher Cognitive Functions

Dopamine is thought to modulate the signal-to-noise ratio in semantic networks (131, 132). The reduction of prefrontal tonic dopaminergic modulation (hypofrontality) in schizophrenia is assumed to decrease the contrast between a signal and noise. This decreased contrast leads to a disinhibited spreading activation within semantic networks [see in Ref. (132) for an overview]. This increased spreading activation would result in remote associations (psychotic symptom) and enhanced semantic priming effects (laboratory measures) in schizophrenia (132). In healthy populations, on the other hand, an experimentally enhanced DA availability (levodopa consumption) focused such priming effects (133). Yet, this dopaminergic modulation of semantic networks differed for the two cerebral hemispheres with a hyperdopaminergia being more prominent in the right than left hemisphere (see previous section). This hyperdopaminergia (potentially resulting from a frontal hypodopaminergia) has been considered to explain the observation that enhanced spreading activation was more relevant for the right than left hemisphere in schizophrenia (134) and healthy individuals high in positive schizotypy (60, 135). Independent studies suggested that these DA-mediated functions explain both these lateralized priming effects and enhanced remote associative processing more generally (136, 137) as well as right-hemisphere shifts of functions in a broader sense. Indeed, the left hemisphere seems compromised in patients with schizophrenia with DA antagonists restoring if not reversing such altered hemispheric asymmetries (138143).

Studies showed that lateralized lexical decision performance is relatively biased toward the right hemisphere in high as compared to low scorers on a positive but not negative schizotypy questionnaire (144, 145). In a pharmacological challenge study, superior left hemisphere language dominance was associated with elevated negative schizotypy scores in a levodopa (but not placebo) group (145). Thus, levodopa might restore left hemisphere language dominance in healthy individuals through (i) the attenuation of the contribution of the right hemisphere to language as a function of positive schizotypy and (ii) an increased contribution of the left hemisphere as a function of negative schizotypy. In an independent sample, but using the same task, the authors calculated signal detection theory measures of sensitivity (d-prime) and response tendency (criterion) (146). Results showed lower d-prime in the levodopa than in the placebo group in individuals low in positive schizotypy. For the response criterion, individuals in the placebo group showed a loosened versus conservative response criterion when being high versus low in positive schizotypy. In the levodopa group, these response tendencies were attenuated. This study suggests that a higher than normal DA availability (i) reduces d-prime in low positive schizotypy individuals and (ii) turns low positive schizotypy individuals less conservative and high positive schizotypy individuals more conservative in their response behavior.

Higher cognitive functions such as working memory and cognitive control have also been studied using experimental psychopharmacological designs. Using a between-subjects, double-blind, placebo-controlled design with groups of high and medium total SPQ scores, amisulpride, a clinically effective antipsychotic with strong D2/D3 receptor antagonistic action, improved working memory and verbal fluency performance in the schizotypy group but impaired it in the medium schizotypy control group (59). The same multi-center study investigated the effects of risperidone, a clinically effective atypical antipsychotic with action on D2 as well as 5-HT receptors, on performance on the antisaccade task, a prominent measure of response inhibition (147, 148). It was found that risperidone impaired antisaccade performance in the medium schizotypy group, whereas performance in the high schizotypy group showed a non-significant tendency toward improvement (149). These findings are in agreement with previous evidence of risperidone impairing antisaccade performance in healthy controls (150) but improving it in patients with schizophrenia (151, 152).

To summarize, findings of this section suggest that individuals with high levels of schizotypy may benefit from DA agonists in terms of cognitive performance. In contrast, individuals with low levels of schizotypy may deteriorate in cognition with DA agonists. Concerning DA antagonists, people with high levels of (particularly positive) schizotypy may benefit in cognitive function from DA antagonistic compounds similar to patients with schizophrenia, or at least tolerate them. People with low or medium levels in (particularly positive) schizotypy may become impaired in these functions. Overall, given the role of DA in the effects of clinically effective antipsychotics (153), the presented data indicate that DA impacts some of the cognitive deficits observed in high (mainly positive) schizotypy (24, 78, 154). However, antipsychotics such as risperidone do not act only on the DA system, thereby limiting the implications for a dopaminergic basis of schizotypy (149).

Schizotypy and Dopamine System Genes

Twin studies have established that individual differences in the various subdimensions of schizotypy in the general population are to a significant part explained by additive genetic factors. Heritability estimates are in the range of 30–50% [see, e.g., Ref. (155157)], although not all studies have found significant heritabilities (158). These behavioral genetic studies, thus, provide a basis for molecular genetic investigations such as those specifically targeting candidate genes related to the DA system.

The most frequently studied gene in the context of schizotypy is the gene coding for catechol-O-methyltransferase, the COMT gene (159161). These studies were originally informed by significant associations of a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the COMT gene with schizophrenia (162); however, later meta-analyses have suggested that this association is not significant (163, 164). The COMT enzyme degrades catecholamines including DA in the synaptic cleft. Due to the paucity of the DA transporter in the prefrontal cortex, COMT plays a particularly prominent role in degrading pre-synaptic DA in that part of the brain. The COMT gene (located in chromosome 22q11.1-q11.2) contains a G to A missense mutation, resulting in a substitution of methionine (met) for valine (val) at codon 158 of the membrane-bound isoform of the protein (reference sequence identification code rs4680). This allelic variation (val158met) is a functional polymorphism: The met158 allele has about one third to one fourth of the activity of the val158 allele, resulting in less efficient catecholamine catabolism and, therefore, higher DA levels.

A number of published studies have reported associations between schizotypy and the COMT val158met polymorphism in healthy subjects. A series of publications from a large-scale study of apparently healthy young men, the ASPIS study, has provided evidence that the high-activity Val allele may be associated with schizotypy. In particular, the Val allele was found to be associated with elevated total SPQ and Perceptual Aberration Scale scores in a first analysis of 379 subjects (165). In a subsequent study using an extended sample of 543 individuals and a factor analysis of SPQ into four factors (cognitive-perceptual, negative, disorganization and paranoia) the Val loading was associated with increased total SPQ and increased negative and disorganized factor scores (71). In a further extension of 908 subjects where the analysis was also performed for individual SPQ dimensions, an association of Val with increased total SPQ, negative disorganized, and paranoia factors was observed. Additionally, there were specific relations of Val loading with increased scores in magical thinking, constricted affect, and odd speech subscales of the SPQ (166).

Broadly confirming this evidence, Grant et al. (167) observed in an independent sample of 280 healthy volunteers that Val/Val homozygotes had significantly higher positive schizotypy scores (O-LIFE unusual experiences) than Met carriers.

Other studies obtained evidence of an association between the Val allele and increased schizotypy in samples of relatives of psychiatric patients. One study (168) observed that among 81 first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients the Val allele was associated with high negative schizotypy (social and physical anhedonia), while no association was found in 38 relatives of patients with bipolar disorder or in 30 healthy controls. Schürhoff et al. (70) studied COMT rs4680 and schizotypy in a combined sample of relatives of schizophrenic patients, relatives of bipolar patients and healthy controls (total N = 106). The authors observed higher total, positive and negative SPQ schizotypy (but not disorganization) scores in association with an increasing number of Val alleles.

Overall, these studies suggest that the Val allele is associated with higher levels of various schizotypy subdimensions. However, evidence to the contrary as well as failures to replicate have also been published. For example, Sheldrick and colleagues (169) reported higher SPQ-B Disorganization scores in the COMT Met/Met group than in the Val/Val group (N = 522 volunteers). Also, no significant association of overall schizotypy with rs4680 genotype was observed in individuals with velo-cardio-facial syndrome (170) or in a mixed sample of members from families with bipolar disorder as well as unaffected controls (total N = 222) (171). Ma and colleagues (172) similarly reported a lack of significant associations between rs4680 and SPQ total and subscale scores in 465 Chinese participants. Finally, Ettinger et al. (173) found no statistically significant association of positive schizotypy (RISC) with rs4680 in a small sample of healthy males (N = 31).

These inconsistencies remain to be resolved, both through additional original studies and a comprehensive meta-analysis that also addresses potential moderator variables. A number of factors may play a role, including the choice of schizotypy questionnaire (174) as well as the ethnic and sociodemographic composition of the sample (175). Other moderating variables may also be important. For example, the above mentioned study by Savitz and colleagues (171) observed, in the absence of an overall association of rs4680 with STA schizotypy, that there was an interaction between childhood trauma (as measured with the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire) and rs4680 on STA: The genotype was associated with STA only in individuals with higher trauma scores, such that Val/Val individuals showed increasing STA with increasing trauma, whereas other genotype groups did not. The possibility of interactive effects of genes and the environment is intriguing and needs to be examined in more detail.

In addition to COMT, a number of other DA-related genes have been investigated in relation to schizotypy. Ettinger et al. (173) obtained no evidence of significant associations of positive schizotypy (RISC) with polymorphisms in the DRD3, DRD4, and SLC6A3 genes in a sample of 31 healthy subjects. Grant et al. (167), however, reported significant associations of different O-LIFE subscales with DRD2 Taq1A, MAOA-uVNTR and SLC6A3 in 288 participants. Finally, a recent study by Taurisano et al. (176) observed an association between SPQ total score and DRD2 rs1076560 in 67 subjects, with GT heterozygotes showing higher scores than G allele homozygotes.

Overall, the discussed molecular genetic findings are promising but must be considered preliminary; they require replication and further investigation in larger samples. Genetic research has the possibility of informing the molecular basis of inter-individual variation in schizotypy and as such makes a unique, non-invasive contribution to the neuroscientific method arsenal available to schizotypy researchers. An additional appeal of this method is the possibility of combining genetic data with other neuroscience methods, such as functional and structural neuroimaging (177).

Molecular Imaging of the Dopamine System in Relation to Schizotypy

Measures of the DA system in humans can also be obtained in vivo using molecular imaging techniques such as PET and SPECT. These methods have been applied successfully to the study of the dopaminergic basis of schizophrenia (45). In contrast, relatively few PET or SPECT studies have investigated the relationship between DA system markers and schizotypy.

An early 123I-IBZM single photon emission tomography investigation (178) reported a significant negative relationship between Eysenckian psychoticism scores and D2 receptor binding in the basal ganglia (relative to frontal cortex) in a small sample of healthy volunteers. Extending this first evidence, a [11C]raclopride PET study (179) found lower D2 receptor density in putamen to be associated with higher scores on the Karolinska Scales of Personality detachment scale, a measure tapping aspects of negative schizotypy. Breier and colleagues replicated this finding, reporting a negative association between the Karolinska detachment scale and striatal D2 receptor binding using [11C]raclopride PET in an independent sample of healthy volunteers (180).

In contrast, a more recent SPECT study observed a positive relationship between the disorganization score of the SPQ and D2/3 receptor binding in the right striatum (relative to occipital cortex) (181). Most recently, Taurisano et al. (176) used SPECT and found a positive correlation between D2 receptor binding in the right putamen and total SPQ score in individuals heterozygous for the DRD2 rs1076560 genotype but not in individuals homozygous for the G allele, suggesting interactive effects of genotype and D2 availability on schizotypal personality. No association between whole striatum DA synthesis capacity and total SPQ score was observed in a PET study of subjects with persistent auditory verbal hallucinations (182).

A different line of molecular imaging studies has focused on dopaminergic challenges in relation to schizotypy. It has previously been shown that patients with schizophrenia, both in the acute phase and in remission, exhibit amphetamine-induced reductions in D2 and D3 DA receptor binding potential in the striatum (40, 183). This result suggests increased striatal DA release in schizophrenia and complements the evidence of heightened pre-synaptic DA function in this disorder (45). Interestingly, a similar pattern of increased striatal amphetamine-induced DA release has been observed in SPD (51). Further evidence confirming and extending this pattern across the schizophrenia spectrum comes from a [18F]fallypride PET study (184). That study showed significant correlations between striatal DA release after a single administration of amphetamine and SPQ total and disorganized dimension scores in healthy volunteers (184). Together, these studies provide evidence for a common striatal dopaminergic dysregulation in both schizotypy and schizophrenia.

Finally, based on evidence of stress as a possible risk factor for schizophrenia (185), molecular imaging methods have been employed to investigate the effects of experimentally induced stress on DA turnover as a function of schizotypy. Using [11C]raclopride PET, it was shown that participants with high levels of negative schizotypy – but not controls or participants with high levels of positive schizotypy – showed a significant stress-induced striatal reduction in binding potential (186).

Overall, studies using molecular imaging methods such as PET or SPECT are able to provide relatively direct markers of the DA system in vivo and, as such, can be used profitably to investigate its relationship not only to schizophrenia but also to schizotypy. However, evidence of associations between DA markers and levels of schizotypy in healthy participants is still scarce. To summarize, while there are associations between lower D2 receptor availability and higher psychoticism and detachment, there is also a report of an association between lower D2/3 receptor binding and lower disorganized dimension schizotypy. This pattern may imply differential associations of DA levels with different dimensions of schizotypy. Finally, there is evidence of increased striatal DA release following amphetamine administration or stress induction in schizotypy.

Conclusions

In this overview, we discussed studies linking DA and schizotypy. We first introduced the neurotransmitter DA and sketched some in vivo methods of its study in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. We presented evidence from the behavioral literature showing that high levels of schizotypy (in particular positive schizotypy) are associated with DA-sensitive functions, in line with behavioral tendencies previously reported in schizophrenia. In addition, we presented evidence that both increased (DA agonists) and decreased (DA antagonists) DA availability seems to be beneficial to behavioral deficits in high schizotypes (mainly positive schizotypy). Those low in schizotypy, however, seemed to deteriorate in their behavioral performance following antipsychotic treatment. While only a careful conjecture, it seems that an increase as well as a decrease of DA may restore function in high schizotypes but deteriorate it in low schizotypes. Overall, we conclude that variance in schizotypy may be explained in part by alterations to the DA system. Of course, as noted, the DA system does not act in isolation but in constant and complex interactions with other neurotransmitter systems.

An underdeveloped theoretical point not only here, but also in schizotypy research more generally, is a well-recognized framework integrating empirical evidence on links between DA and schizotypy from various levels of analysis (not only concerning DA) into a coherent theory. Important theoretical contributions by Meehl (8, 9) and others (16, 187) have influenced our current thinking on schizotypy. A significant body of new data has been amassed that needs to be theoretically linked. In particular, we think of the psychopharmacological, imaging and genetic findings that are based on techniques that had not been available before. To achieve this aim, it may be instructive to turn in two directions for inspiration, viz. in the direction of (i) personality theory in individual differences research and (ii) clinical schizophrenia research.

Regarding individual differences, DeYoung (188) recently integrated previous assumptions into a theoretical framework that is relevant to our discussion of the role of DA in schizotypy. Particularly relevant is his discussion of “apophenia,” i.e., individuals’ tendency for overinclusive thinking and perception. Conrad (189) termed apophenia as the “unmotivated seeing of connections” accompanied by a “specific experience of an abnormal meaningfulness” (p. 46). For instance, one can refer to apophenia when individuals see and create pattern in random noise, whether this noise is perceptual (190, 191), semantic (136), or probabilistic (192). The suggestion of a link between apophenia and positive schizotypy in particular (136, 193) including its modulation by DA (60, 194) is not new. However, DeYoung (188) links apophenia not only with positive schizotypy and the positive symptoms of psychosis but adds another link, namely that apophenia can be predicted by openness, one of the personality traits of the five factor model of personality (195). Accordingly, apophenia can be called “openness to implausible patterns” [Ref. (188); p. 13]. This openness and its link to apophenia are hypothesized to stem from high-activity levels in the dopaminergic salience system. As mentioned above (44), recent theories on the role of DA in schizophrenia suggest that stressors dysregulate DA at the pre-synaptic control level. This is hypothesized to lead to changes in the appraisal of events, making them more salient (i.e., causing apophenia). Thus, DeYoung’s (188) DA theory of personality is of interest as it ties schizotypy in with other personality traits and establishes parallels with schizophrenia at the cognitive (overinclusive pattern detection) and neurophysiological (DA) level.

These parallels link directly to the second direction of inspiration, i.e., clinical schizophrenia research. Here, aberrant salience has become a predominant topic. Current theorizing has provided promising explanations as to how DA links with the symptomatic expression of the clinical condition (153, 196). Specifically, these theories start to fill the explanatory gap between an apparent neurochemical disturbance in the brain and the formation of complex delusions and hallucinations. The argument is that delusions arise as a result of individuals’ explanations of experienced aberrant salience. This argument is based on the role of DA in mediating both the presence or absence of reward and the salience, such as novelty, of stimuli. Increased DA neurotransmission is thought to lead to aberrant salience, that is, the direction of attention to objectively irrelevant internal or external stimuli. Delusions are by this account thought to arise through individuals’ attempts to explain this distressing experience, likely in interaction with prior experiences and beliefs, both personal and socio-cultural. While it is not clear yet which aspect of salience is particularly disturbed in schizophrenia, this work provides a promising model for understanding an important symptom of schizophrenia. Additionally, it remains unclear from this work how other symptoms of schizophrenia, such as hallucinations, thought disorder and negative symptoms arise. Because we know of numerous established similarities between high schizotypy and schizophrenia [see in Ref. (24) for a recent overview], such theories may be drawn upon for our understanding of schizotypy as well.

In sum, we conclude that there is some evidence of an association between altered DA neurotransmission and schizotypy, particularly positive schizotypy. Such work informs not only neurobiological, but also cognitive models of the schizophrenia spectrum (196) and potential neuropharmacological treatments (197).

Future Work

The current overview illustrates the numerous and varied ways in which researchers have aimed to investigate the link between schizotypy and DA. The heterogeneity in the approaches and the sometimes conflicting results make it difficult to arrive at a clear-cut conclusion. Most of the time, we cannot directly compare results across study domains. This heterogeneity is of course frustrating but may also be constructive in providing directions as to where more work needs to be done.

Specific suggestions for future work include (1) the need to continue working on the development of an overarching theory of schizotypy (2), the assessment of the direction of effects of increasing and decreasing DA availability in high and low schizotypy of different dimensions (3), the role of concomitant illegal and legal drug use (4), the role of individuals’ genetic makeup, and (5) the identification of schizotypal markers that are truly unfavorable concerning high-risk states and those that are potentially of little psychopathological impact.

In our view, the field needs both carefully controlled laboratory experiments and studies that account for the spontaneous self-administration of psychoactive ingredients that frequently act on the DA system. For instance, there is insufficient knowledge as to why schizotypal individuals and patients with schizophrenia consume DA-sensitive drugs to a higher degree than controls (198200). It remains to be investigated whether this drug consumption is a potential trigger for psychosis or whether such individuals medicate themselves by altering their DA systems.

Also, inconsistencies in the literature remain regarding the molecular genetics of schizotypy. Future studies are needed to provide firmer answers on the important question of the molecular genetic makeup of schizotypy. Given the advance in sample sizes in the genetics of schizophrenia and the likely small sizes of genetic effects on schizophrenia spectrum phenotypes (201), it will be important to substantially increase sample sizes in the study of molecular genetic correlates of schizotypy. A promising way to achieve appropriate sample sizes is the establishment of multi-site collaborations under the guidance of consortia.

An additional avenue for future work concerns the direct comparison between individuals with high levels of schizotypy and patients with schizophrenia. While differences between individuals with high and low schizotypy scores have been shown on numerous occasions, much less is known about differences between highly schizotypal individuals and schizophrenia patients. Such a comparison would allow the identification of factors that perhaps protect schizotypes from developing the full-blown clinical condition (24).

Finally, it should be pointed out as a limitation that this overview provides merely a qualitative discussion of the evidence of an association between DA and schizotypy. An important future contribution to this literature would be a formal meta-analysis of the size and significance of this association across different studies and methods.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Rado S. Dynamics and classification of disordered behaviour. Am J Psychiatry (1953) 110:406–16.

Google Scholar

2. Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull (1991) 17:555–64. doi: 10.1093/schbul/17.4.555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Mason O, Claridge G. The Oxford–Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE): further description and extended norms. Schizophr Res (2006) 82:203–11. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.12.845

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Chapman LJ, Kwapil TR. Scales for the measurement of schizotypy. In: Raine A, Lencz T, Mednick SA, editors. Schizotypal Personality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1995). p. 79–106.

Google Scholar

5. Rust J. The Rust Inventory of Schizotypal Cognitions (RISC). Schizophr Bull (1988) 14:317–22. doi:10.1093/schbul/14.2.317

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Raine A. Schizotypal personality: neurodevelopmental and psychosocial trajectories. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2006) 2:291–326. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095318

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Liddle PF. The symptoms of chronic schizophrenia. A re-examination of the positive-negative dichotomy. Br J Psychiatry (1987) 151:145–51. doi:10.1192/bjp.151.2.145

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Meehl PE. Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia. J Pers Disord (1990) 4:1–99. doi:10.1521/pedi.1990.4.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Meehl PE. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1962) 46:935–44. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810100077015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Meehl PE. Schizotaxia revisited. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1989) 46:935–44. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810100077015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Lenzenweger MF. Schizotypy and Schizophrenia: The View from Experimental Psychopathology. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2010).

Google Scholar

12. Haslam N, Holland E, Kuppens P. Categories versus dimensions in personality and psychopathology: a quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychol Med (2012) 42:903–20. doi:10.1017/S0033291711001966

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Rawlings D, Williams B, Haslam N, Claridge G. Taxometric analysis supports a dimensional latent trait structure for schizotypy. Pers Indiv Differ (2008) 44:1640–51. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Claridge G. Schizotypy: Implications for Illness and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1997).

Google Scholar

15. Claridge G. The schizophrenias as nervous types. Br J Psychiatry (1972) 121:1–17. doi:10.1192/bjp.121.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Claridge G. ‘The schizophrenias as nervous types’ revisited. Br J Psychiatry (1987) 151:735–43. doi:10.1192/bjp.151.6.735

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Eysenck HJ. The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield: Thomas (1967).

Google Scholar

18. Eysenck SB, Eysenck HJ. The measurement of psychoticism: a study of factor stability and reliability. Br J Soc Clin Psychol (1968) 7:286–94. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1968.tb00571.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Barrett TR, Etheridge JB. Verbal hallucinations in normals, I: people who hear “voices.”. Appl Cogn Psychol (1992) 6:379–87. doi:10.1002/acp.2350060503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lincoln TM. Relevant dimensions of delusions: continuing the continuum versus category debate. Schizophr Res (2007) 93:211–20. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Johns LC, van Os J. The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general population. Clin Psychol Rev (2001) 21:1125–41. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00103-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Barrantes-Vidal N, Gross GM, Sheinbaum T, Mitjavila M, Ballespí S, Kwapil TR. Positive and negative schizotypy are associated with prodromal and schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms. Schizophr Res (2013) 145:50–5. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.01.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Nelson MT, Seal ML, Pantelis C, Phillips LJ. Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2013) 37:317–27. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Ettinger U, Meyhöfer I, Steffens M, Wagner M, Koutsouleris N. Genetics, cognition, and neurobiology of schizotypal personality: a review of the overlap with schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry (2014) 5:18. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Davis KL, Kahn RS, Ko G, Davidson M. Dopamine in schizophrenia: a review and reconceptualization. Am J Psychiatry (1991) 148:1474–86.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, van Dyck CH, Gil R, D’Souza CD, Erdos J, et al. Single photon emission computerized tomography imaging of amphetamine-induced dopamine release in drug-free schizophrenic subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1996) 93:9235–40. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.17.9235

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Banich MT. Banich, Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company (2004).

Google Scholar

28. Tritsch NX, Sabatini BL. Dopaminergic modulation of synaptic transmission in cortex and striatum. Neuron (2012) 76:33–50. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Mehler-Wex C, Riederer P, Gerlach M. Dopaminergic dysbalance in distinct basal ganglia neurocircuits: implications for the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease, schizophrenia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurotox Res (2006) 10:167–79. doi:10.1007/BF03033354

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Montgomery AJ, Lingford-Hughes AR, Egerton A, Nutt DJ, Grasby PM. The effect of nicotine on striatal dopamine release in man: a [11C]raclopride PET study. Synapse (2007) 61:637–45. doi:10.1002/syn.20419

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Javitt DC. Glutamate and schizophrenia: phencyclidine, N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors, and dopamine-glutamate interactions. Int Rev Neurobiol (2007) 78:69–108. doi:10.1016/S0074-7742(06)78003-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kuczenski R, Segal DS. Exposure of adolescent rats to oral methylphenidate: preferential effects on extracellular norepinephrine and absence of sensitization and cross-sensitization to methamphetamine. J Neurosci (2002) 22:7264–71.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Rothman RB, Baumann MH, Dersch CM, Romero DV, Rice KC, Carroll FI, et al. Amphetamine-type central nervous system stimulants release norepinephrine more potently than they release dopamine and serotonin. Synapse (2001) 39:32–41. doi:10.1002/1098-2396(20010101)39:1<32::AID-SYN5>3.0.CO;2-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Stip E. Happy birthday neuroleptics! 50 years later: la folie du doute. Eur Psychiatry (2002) 17:115–9. doi:10.1016/S0924-9338(02)00639-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Klein D, Davis J. Diagnosis and Drug Treatment of Psychiatric Disorders. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins (1969).

Google Scholar

36. Matthysse S. Antipsychotic drug actions: a clue to the neuropathology of schizophrenia? Fed Proc (1973) 32:200–5.

Google Scholar

37. Kapur S, Mamo D. Half a century of antipsychotics and still a central role for dopamine D2 receptors. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2003) 27:1081–90. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2003.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A. Dopamine as the wind of the psychotic fire: new evidence from brain imaging studies. J Psychopharmacol (1999) 13:358–71. doi:10.1177/026988119901300405

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Seeman P. Schizophrenia and dopamine receptors. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2013) 23:999–1009. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Abi-Dargham A, Gil R, Krystal J, Baldwin RM, Seibyl JP, Bowers M, et al. Increased striatal dopamine transmission in schizophrenia: confirmation in a second cohort. Am J Psychiatry (1998) 155:761–7.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Davidson M, Keefe RS, Mohs RC, Siever LJ, Losonczy MF, Horvath TB, et al. l-Dopa challenge and relapse in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (1987) 144:934–8.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Janowsky DS, Risch C. Amphetamine psychosis and psychotic symptoms. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (1979) 65:73–7. doi:10.1007/BF00491982

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Sekine Y, Iyo M, Ouchi Y, Matsunaga T, Tsukada H, Okada H, et al. Methamphetamine-related psychiatric symptoms and reduced brain dopamine transporters studied with PET. Am J Psychiatry (2001) 158:1206–14. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1206

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Howes OD, Kapur S. The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III – the final common pathway. Schizophr Bull (2009) 35:549–62. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Howes OD, Kambeitz J, Kim E, Stahl D, Slifstein M, Abi-Dargham A, et al. The nature of dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia and what this means for treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2012) 69:776–86. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.169

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Thompson JL, Urban N, Slifstein M, Xu X, Kegeles LS, Girgis RR, et al. Striatal dopamine release in schizophrenia comorbid with substance dependence. Mol Psychiatry (2013) 18:909–15. doi:10.1038/mp.2012.109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Papanastasiou E, Stone JM, Shergill S. When the drugs don’t work: the potential of glutamatergic antipsychotics in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry (2013) 202:91–3. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.112.110999

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Howes OD, Montgomery AJ, Asselin MC, Murray RM, Valli I, Tabraham P, et al. Elevated striatal dopamine function linked to prodromal signs of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2009) 66:13–20. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.514

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Allen P, Modinos G, Hubl D, Shields G, Cachia A, Jardri R, et al. Neuroimaging auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: from neuroanatomy to neurochemistry and beyond. Schizophr Bull (2012) 38:695–703. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs066

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Howes O, Bose S, Turkheimer F, Valli I, Egerton A, Stahl D, et al. Progressive increase in striatal dopamine synthesis capacity as patients develop psychosis: a PET study. Mol Psychiatry (2011) 16:885–6. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Abi-Dargham A, Kegeles LS, Zea-Ponce Y, Mawlawi O, Martinez D, Mitropoulou V, et al. Striatal amphetamine-induced dopamine release in patients with schizotypal personality disorder studied with single photon emission computed tomography and [123I]iodobenzamide. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 55:1001–6. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.01.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Egerton A, Chaddock CA, Winton-Brown TT, Bloomfield MA, Bhattacharyya S, Allen P, et al. Presynaptic striatal dopamine dysfunction in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis: findings in a second cohort. Biol Psychiatry (2013) 74:106–12. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Huttunen J, Heinimaa M, Svirskis T, Nyman M, Kajander J, Forsback S, et al. Striatal dopamine synthesis in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 63:114–7. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Brunelin J, d’Amato T, van Os J, Cochet A, Suaud-Chagny MF, Saoud M. Effects of acute metabolic stress on the dopaminergic and pituitary-adrenal axis activity in patients with schizophrenia, their unaffected siblings and controls. Schizophr Res (2008) 100:206–11. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Myin-Germeys I, Marcelis M, Krabbendam L, Delespaul P, van Os J. Subtle fluctuations in psychotic phenomena as functional states of abnormal dopamine reactivity in individuals at risk. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 58:105–10. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kuepper R, Ceccarini J, Lataster J, van Os J, van Kroonenburgh M, van Gerven JM, et al. Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol-induced dopamine release as a function of psychosis risk: 18F-fallypride positron emission tomography study. PLoS One (2013) 8:e70378. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0070378

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Nordstrom AL, Farde L, Halldin C. Time course of D2-dopamine receptor occupancy examined by PET after single oral doses of haloperidol. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (1992) 106:433–8. doi:10.1007/BF02244811

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Leucht S, Cipriani A, Spineli L, Mavridis D, Orey D, Richter F, et al. Comparative efficacy and tolerability of 15 antipsychotic drugs in schizophrenia: a multiple-treatments meta-analysis. Lancet (2013) 382:951–62. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(13)60733-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Koychev I, McMullen K, Lees J, Dadhiwala R, Grayson L, Perry C, et al. A validation of cognitive biomarkers for the early identification of cognitive enhancing agents in schizotypy: a three-center double-blind placebo-controlled study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2012) 22:469–81. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Mohr C, Landis T, Brugger P. Lateralized semantic priming: modulation by levodopa, semantic distance, and participants’ magical beliefs. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat (2006) 2:71–84.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Schmechtig A, Lees J, Perkins A, Altavilla A, Craig KJ, Dawson GR, et al. The effects of ketamine and risperidone on eye movement control in healthy volunteers. Transl Psychiatry (2013) 3:e334. doi:10.1038/tp.2013.109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Weiner I, Arad M. Using the pharmacology of latent inhibition to model domains of pathology in schizophrenia and their treatment. Behav Brain Res (2009) 204:369–86. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.05.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Braff DL. Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex: a window on the brain in schizophrenia. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2010) 4:349–71. doi:10.1007/7854_2010_61

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Karson CN. Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic systems. Brain (1983) 106(Pt 3):643–53. doi:10.1093/brain/106.3.643

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Bracha HS, Livingston RL, Clothier J, Linington BB, Karson CN. Correlation of severity of psychiatric patients’ delusions with right hemispatial inattention (left-turning behavior). Am J Psychiatry (1993) 150:330–2.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Mohr C, Landis T, Sandor PS, Fathi M, Brugger P. Nonstereotyped responding in positive schizotypy after a single dose of levodopa. Neuropsychopharmacology (2004) 29:1741–51. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300500

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Kopp B, Wolff M, Hruska C, Reischies FM. Brain mechanisms of visual encoding and working memory in psychometrically identified schizotypal individuals and after acute administration of haloperidol. Psychophysiology (2002) 39:459–72. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3940459

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Kumari V, Cotter PA, Mulligan OF, Checkley SA, Gray NS, Hemsley DR, et al. Effects of d-amphetamine and haloperidol on latent inhibition in healthy male volunteers. J Psychopharmacol (1999) 13:398–405. doi:10.1177/026988119901300411

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Ridley RM, Baker HF, Frith CD, Dowdy J, Crow TJ. Stereotyped responding on a two-choice guessing task by marmosets and humans treated with amphetamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (1988) 95:560–4. doi:10.1007/BF00172977

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Schürhoff F, Szöke A, Chevalier F, Roy I, Méary A, Bellivier F, et al. Schizotypal dimensions: an intermediate phenotype associated with the COMT high activity allele. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet (2007) 144:64–8. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30395

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Stefanis NC, Van Os J, Avramopoulos D, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Hantoumi I, et al. Variation in catechol-o-methyltransferase val158 met genotype associated with schizotypy but not cognition: a population study in 543 young men. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 56:510–5. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.038

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Brugger P, Graves RE. Right hemispatial inattention and magical ideation. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (1997) 247:55–7. doi:10.1007/BF02916254

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Gray NS, Pickering AD, Snowden RJ, Hemsley DR, Gray JA. The partial reinforcement extinction effect in humans: effects of schizophrenia, schizotypy and low doses of amphetamine. Behav Brain Res (2002) 133:333–42. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00019-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Mohr C, Bracha HS, Brugger P. Magical ideation modulates spatial behavior. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2003) 15:168–74. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.15.2.168

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Williams JH, Wellman NA, Geaney DP, Feldon J, Cowen PJ, Rawlins JN. Haloperidol enhances latent inhibition in visual tasks in healthy people. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (1997) 133:262–8. doi:10.1007/s002130050400

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Pycock CJ. Experimental model of hemi-parkinsonism. In: Myslobodsky MS, editor. Hemisyndromes: Psychobiology, Neurology, Psychiatry. New York, NY: Academic Press (1983). p. 69–90.

Google Scholar

77. Bracha HS, Shults C, Glick SD, Kleinman JE. Spontaneous asymmetric circling behavior in hemi-parkinsonism; a human equivalent of the lesioned-circling rodent behavior. Life Sci (1987) 40:1127–30. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(87)90576-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Mohr C, Landis T, Bracha HS, Fathi M, Brugger P. Levodopa reverses gait asymmetries related to anhedonia and magical ideation. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2005) 255:33–9. doi:10.1007/s00406-004-0531-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Chermahini SA, Hommel B. The (b)link between creativity and dopamine: spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition (2010) 115:458–65. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Ettinger U, Klein C. Eye movements. In: Reuter M, Montag C, editors. Neuroeconomics. Berlin: Springer Verlag (in press).

Google Scholar

81. Jacobsen LK, Hommer DW, Hong WL, Castellanos FX, Frazier JA, Giedd JN, et al. Blink rate in childhood-onset schizophrenia: comparison with normal and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder controls. Biol Psychiatry (1996) 40:1222–9. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(95)00630-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Mackert A, Woyth C, Flechtner KM, Volz HP. Increased blink rate in drug-naive acute schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry (1990) 27:1197–202. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(90)90417-Z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Chen EY, Lam LC, Chen RY, Nguyen DG. Blink rate, neurocognitive impairments, and symptoms in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (1996) 40:597–603. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(95)00482-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Blin O, Masson G, Azulay JP, Fondarai J, Serratrice G. Apomorphine-induced blinking and yawning in healthy volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol (1990) 30:769–73. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2125.1990.tb03848.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Strakowski SM, Sax KW, Setters MJ, Keck PE Jr. Enhanced response to repeated d-amphetamine challenge: evidence for behavioral sensitization in humans. Biol Psychiatry (1996) 40:872–80. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(95)00497-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Mohr C, Sándor PS, Landis T, Fathi M, Brugger P. Blinking and schizotypal thinking. J Psychopharmacol (2005) 19:513–20. doi:10.1177/0269881105056538

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Colzato LS, Slagter HA, van den Wildenberg WPM, Hommel B. Closing one’s eyes to reality: evidence for a dopaminergic basis of Psychoticism from spontaneous eye blink rates. Pers Indiv Differ (2009) 46:377–80. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Ettinger U, Corr PJ, Mofidi A, Williams SC, Kumari V. Dopaminergic basis of the psychosis-prone personality investigated with functional magnetic resonance imaging of procedural learning. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:130. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00130

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Corr PJ. Psychoticism. In: Kazdin AE, editor. Encyclopedia of Psychology. Washington, DC: Oxford University Press/APA (2000). p. 469–70.

Google Scholar

90. Howarth E. What does Eysenck’s psychoticism scale really measure? Br J Psychol (1986) 77(Pt 2):223–7. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb01996.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Farmer A, Redman K, Harris T, Webb R, Mahmood A, Sadler S, et al. A sib-pair study of psychoticism, life events and depression. Pers Indiv Differ (2003) 34:613–23. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00036-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Kraepelin E. Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. New York, NY: RE Krieger (1919).

Google Scholar

93. Peralta V, Cuesta MJ. Motor features in psychotic disorders. I. Factor structure and clinical correlates. Schizophr Res (2001) 47:107–16. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00035-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Jones IH. Observations on schizophrenic stereotypies. Compr Psychiatry (1965) 6:323–35. doi:10.1016/S0010-440X(65)80026-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Perry W, Braff DL. A multimethod approach to assessing perseverations in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res (1998) 33:69–77. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00061-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Cadenhead KS, Perry W, Shafer K, Braff DL. Cognitive functions in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res (1999) 37:123–32. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00147-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Raine A, Sheard C, Reynolds GP, Lencz T. Pre-frontal structural and functional deficits associated with individual differences in schizotypal personality. Schizophr Res (1992) 7:237–47. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(92)90018-Z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Gooding DC, Kwapil TR, Tallent KA. Wisconsin card sorting test deficits in schizotypic individuals. Schizophr Res (1999) 40:201–9. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00124-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Lenzenweger MF, Jensen ST, Rubin DB. Finding the “genuine” schizotype: a model and method for resolving heterogeneity in performance on laboratory measures in experimental psychopathology research. J Abnorm Psychol (2003) 112:457–68. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.457

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Brugger P. Variables that influence the generation of random sequences: an update. Percept Mot Skills (1997) 84:627–61. doi:10.2466/pms.1997.84.2.627

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Salame P, Danion JM, Peretti S, Cuervo C. The state of functioning of working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (1998) 30:11–29. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00107-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Avons SE, Nunn JA, Chan L, Armstrong H. Executive function assessed by memory updating and random generation in schizotypal individuals. Psychiatry Res (2003) 120:145–54. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00174-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Brugger P, Landis T, Regard M. A sheep goat effect in repetition avoidance – extra-sensory perception as an effect of subjective-probability. Br J Psychol (1990) 81:455–68. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02372.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Graham FK. Presidential Address, 1974. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimulation. Psychophysiology (1975) 12:238–48. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1975.tb01284.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL. Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2001) 156:194–215. doi:10.1007/s002130100799

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Braff DL. Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (1993) 19:233–59. doi:10.1093/schbul/19.2.233

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Cadenhead KS, Swerdlow NR, Shafer KM, Diaz M, Braff DL. Modulation of the startle response and startle laterality in relatives of schizophrenic patients and in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of inhibitory deficits. Am J Psychiatry (2000) 157:1660–8. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1660

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Varty GB, Higgins GA. Examination of drug-induced and isolation-induced disruptions of prepulse inhibition as models to screen antipsychotic drugs. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (1995) 122:15–26. doi:10.1007/BF02246437

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Swerdlow NR, Bakshi V, Waikar M, Taaid N, Geyer MA. Seroquel, clozapine and chlorpromazine restore sensorimotor gating in ketamine-treated rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (1998) 140:75–80. doi:10.1007/s002130050741

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Frau R, Orrù M, Puligheddu M, Gessa GL, Mereu G, Marrosu F, et al. Sleep deprivation disrupts prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex: reversal by antipsychotic drugs. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2008) 11:947–55. doi:10.1017/S1461145708008900

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Petrovsky N, Ettinger U, Hill A, Frenzel L, Meyhöfer I, Wagner M, et al. Sleep deprivation disrupts prepulse inhibition and induces psychosis-like symptoms in healthy humans. J Neurosci (2014) 34:9134–40. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0904-14.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Li M, He E, Volf N. Time course of the attenuation effect of repeated antipsychotic treatment on prepulse inhibition disruption induced by repeated phencyclidine treatment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (2011) 98:559–69. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2011.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Kumari V, Soni W, Sharma T. Normalization of information processing deficits in schizophrenia with clozapine. Am J Psychiatry (1999) 156:1046–51.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Kumari V, Sharma T. Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia: a critical evaluation of current evidence and directions for future research. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2002) 162:97–101. doi:10.1007/s00213-002-1099-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Oranje B, Van Oel CJ, Gispen-De Wied CC, Verbaten MN, Kahn RS. Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on the prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in patients with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2002) 22:359–65. doi:10.1097/00004714-200208000-00005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Leumann L, Feldon J, Vollenweider FX, Ludewig K. Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on prepulse inhibition and latent inhibition in chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 52:729–39. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01344-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Quednow BB, Wagner M, Westheide J, Beckmann K, Bliesener N, Maier W, et al. Sensorimotor gating and habituation of the startle response in schizophrenic patients randomly treated with amisulpride or olanzapine. Biol Psychiatry (2006) 59:536–45. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Sprock J, Hsieh MH, Braff DL. Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2006) 63:1325–35. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1325

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Sprock J, Calkins ME, Green MF, Greenwood TA, et al. Deficient prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia detected by the multi-site COGS. Schizophr Res (2014) 152:503–12. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.12.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Takahashi H, Iwase M, Canuet L, Yasuda Y, Ohi K, Fukumoto M, et al. Relationship between prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle response and schizotypy in healthy Japanese subjects. Psychophysiology (2010) 47:831–7. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01000.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Swerdlow NR, Filion D, Geyer MA, Braff DL. “Normal” personality correlates of sensorimotor, cognitive, and visuospatial gating. Biol Psychiatry (1995) 37:286–99. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(94)00138-S

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Kumari V, Antonova E, Geyer MA. Prepulse inhibition and “psychosis-proneness” in healthy individuals: an fMRI study. Eur Psychiatry (2008) 23:274–80. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.11.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Kumari V, Gray JA, Geyer MA, ffytche D, Soni W, Mitterschiffthaler MT, et al. Neural correlates of tactile prepulse inhibition: a functional MRI study in normal and schizophrenic subjects. Psychiatry Res (2003) 122:99–113. doi:10.1016/S0925-4927(02)00123-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2001) 156:234–58. doi:10.1007/s002130100810

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Kumari V, Ettinger U. Prepulse inhibition deficits in schizophrenia: static or amenable to treatment? In: Lang MV, editor. Progress in Schizophrenia Research. New York, NY: Nova Publishers (2004). p. 95–117.

Google Scholar

126. Völter C, Riedel M, Wöstmann N, Aichert DS, Lobo S, Costa A, et al. Sensorimotor gating and D2 receptor signalling: evidence from a molecular genetic approach. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2012) 15:1427–40. doi:10.1017/S1461145711001787

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Weiner I. The “two-headed” latent inhibition model of schizophrenia: modeling positive and negative symptoms and their treatment. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2003) 169:257–97. doi:10.1007/s00213-002-1313-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Lubow RE, Gewirtz JC. Latent inhibition in humans: data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia. Psychol Bull (1995) 117:87–103. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.87

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Kumari V, Ettinger U. Latent inhibition in schizophrenia and schizotypy: a review of the empirical literature. In: Lubow RE, editor. Latent Inhibition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2010). p. 419–47.

Google Scholar

130. Evans LH, Gray NS, Snowden RJ. A new continuous within-participants latent inhibition task: examining associations with schizotypy dimensions, smoking status and gender. Biol Psychol (2007) 74:365–73. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.09.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Servan-Schreiber D, Printz H, Cohen JD. A network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science (1990) 249:892–5. doi:10.1126/science.2392679

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Spitzer M. A cognitive neuroscience view of schizophrenic thought disorder. Schizophr Bull (1997) 23:29–50. doi:10.1093/schbul/23.1.29

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Kischka U, Kammer T, Maier S, Weisbrod M, Thimm M, Spitzer M. Dopaminergic modulation of semantic network activation. Neuropsychologia (1996) 34:1107–13. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(96)00024-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Weisbrod M, Maier S, Harig S, Himmelsbach U, Spitzer M. Lateralised semantic and indirect semantic priming effects in people with schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry (1998) 172:142–6. doi:10.1192/bjp.172.2.142

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Pizzagalli D, Lehmann D, Brugger P. Lateralized direct and indirect semantic priming effects in subjects with paranormal experiences and beliefs. Psychopathology (2001) 34:75–80. doi:10.1159/000049284

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Gianotti LR, Mohr C, Pizzagalli D, Lehmann D, Brugger P. Associative processing and paranormal belief. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2001) 55:595–603. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1819.2001.00911.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Mohr C, Graves RE, Gianotti LR, Pizzagalli D, Brugger P. Loose but normal: a semantic association study. J Psycholinguist Res (2001) 30:475–83. doi:10.1023/A:1010461429079

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Levine J, Martine T, Feraro R, Kimhi R, Bracha HS. Medicated chronic schizophrenic patients do not demonstrate left turning asymmetry. Neuropsychobiology (1997) 36:22–4. doi:10.1159/000119355

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Maruff P, Hay D, Malone V, Currie J. Asymmetries in the covert orienting of visual spatial attention in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia (1995) 33:1205–23. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(95)00037-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Purdon SE, Flor-Henry P. Asymmetrical olfactory acuity and neuroleptic treatment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2000) 44:221–32. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00212-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Purdon SE, Woodward ND, Flor-Henry P. Asymmetrical hand force persistence and neuroleptic treatment in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2001) 7:606–14. doi:10.1017/S1355617701755087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Schröder J, Bubeck B, Silvestri S, Demisch S, Sauer H. Gender differences in D2 dopamine receptor binding in drug-naive patients with schizophrenia: an [123I]iodobenzamide single photon emission computed tomography study. Psychiatry Res (1997) 75:115–23. doi:10.1016/S0925-4927(97)00046-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Tomer R, Flor-Henry P. Neuroleptics reverse attention asymmetries in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry (1989) 25:852–60. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(89)90264-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Brugger P, Gamma A, Muri R, Schäfer M, Taylor KI. Functional hemispheric asymmetry and belief in ESP: towards a “neuropsychology of belief”. Percept Mot Skills (1993) 77:1299–308. doi:10.2466/pms.1993.77.3f.1299

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Mohr C, Krummenacher P, Landis T, Sandor PS, Fathi M, Brugger P. Psychometric schizotypy modulates levodopa effects on lateralized lexical decision performance. J Psychiatr Res (2005) 39:241–50. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.08.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Krummenacher P, Mohr C, Haker H, Brugger P. Dopamine, paranormal belief, and the detection of meaningful stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci (2010) 22:1670–81. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21313

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Hutton SB, Ettinger U. The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. Psychophysiology (2006) 43:302–13. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00403.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol (2000) 41:49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Schmechtig A, Lees J, Grayson L, Craig KJ, Dadhiwala R, Dawson GR, et al. Effects of risperidone, amisulpride and nicotine on eye movement control and their modulation by schizotypy. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2013) 227:331–45. doi:10.1007/s00213-013-2973-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Barrett SL, Bell R, Watson D, King DJ. Effects of amisulpride, risperidone and chlorpromazine on auditory and visual latent inhibition, prepulse inhibition, executive function and eye movements in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol (2004) 18:156–72. doi:10.1177/0269881104042614

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Burke JG, Reveley MA. Improved antisaccade performance with risperidone in schizophrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2002) 72:449–54. doi:10.1136/jnnp.72.4.449

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Harris MS, Reilly JL, Keshavan MS, Sweeney JA. Longitudinal studies of antisaccades in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia. Psychol Med (2006) 36:485–94. doi:10.1017/S0033291705006756

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2003) 160:13–23. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Giakoumaki SG. Cognitive and prepulse inhibition deficits in psychometrically high schizotypal subjects in the general population: relevance to schizophrenia research. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2012) 18:643–56. doi:10.1017/S135561771200029X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Claridge G, Hewitt J. A biometrical study of schizotypy in a normal population. Pers Indiv Differ (1987) 8:303–12. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(87)90030-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Linney YM, Murray RM, Peters ER, MacDonald AM, Rijsdijk F, Sham PC. A quantitative genetic analysis of schizotypal personality traits. Psychol Med (2003) 33:803–16. doi:10.1017/S0033291703007906

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Macare C, Bates TC, Heath AC, Martin NG, Ettinger U. Substantial genetic overlap between schizotypy and neuroticism: a twin study. Behav Genet (2012) 42:732–42. doi:10.1007/s10519-012-9558-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. MacDonald AW, Pogue-Geile MF, Debski TT, Manuck S. Genetic and environmental influences on schizotypy: a community-based twin study. Schizophr Bull (2001) 27:47–58. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006859

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Lachman HM, Papolos DF, Saito T, Yu YM, Szumlanski CL, Weinshilboum RM. Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics (1996) 6:243–50. doi:10.1097/00008571-199606000-00007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Weinshilboum RM, Otterness DM, Szumlanski CL. Methylation pharmacogenetics: catechol O-methyltransferase, thiopurine methyltransferase, and histamine N-methyltransferase. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol (1999) 39:19–52. doi:10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.39.1.19

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Montag C, Jurkiewicz M, Reuter M. The role of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene in personality and related psychopathological disorders. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets (2012) 11:236–50. doi:10.2174/187152712800672382

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Mazzanti CM, Straub RE, et al. Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2001) 98:6917–22. doi:10.1073/pnas.111134598

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Munafo MR, Bowes L, Clark TG, Flint J. Lack of association of the COMT (Val158/108 Met) gene and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of case-control studies. Mol Psychiatry (2005) 10:765–70. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001664

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Okochi T, Ikeda M, Kishi T, Kawashima K, Kinoshita Y, Kitajima T, et al. Meta-analysis of association between genetic variants in COMT and schizophrenia: an update. Schizophr Res (2009) 110:140–8. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.02.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Avramopoulos D, Stefanis NC, Hantoumi I, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis CN. Higher scores of self reported schizotypy in healthy young males carrying the COMT high activity allele. Mol Psychiatry (2002) 7:706–11. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Smyrnis N, Avramopoulos D, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis CN, Tsekou H, Stefanis NC. Effect of schizotypy on cognitive performance and its tuning by COMT val158 met genotype variations in a large population of young men. Biol Psychiatry (2007) 61:845–53. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Grant P, Kuepper Y, Mueller EA, Wielpuetz C, Mason O, Hennig J. Dopaminergic foundations of schizotypy as measured by the German version of the Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE)-a suitable endophenotype of schizophrenia. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:1. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Docherty AR, Sponheim SR. Anhedonia as a phenotype for the Val158Met COMT polymorphism in relatives of patients with schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol (2008) 117:788–98. doi:10.1037/a0013745

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Sheldrick AJ, Krug A, Markov V, Leube D, Michel TM, Zerres K, et al. Effect of COMT val158met genotype on cognition and personality. Eur Psychiatry (2008) 23:385–9. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.05.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Murphy KC, Jones LA, Owen MJ. High rates of schizophrenia in adults with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1999) 56:940–5. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.940

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Savitz J, van der Merwe L, Newman TK, Stein DJ, Ramesar R. Catechol-o-methyltransferase genotype and childhood trauma may interact to impact schizotypal personality traits. Behav Genet (2010) 40:415–23. doi:10.1007/s10519-009-9323-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Ma X, Sun J, Yao J, Wang Q, Hu X, Deng W, et al. A quantitative association study between schizotypal traits and COMT, PRODH and BDNF genes in a healthy Chinese population. Psychiatry Res (2007) 153:7–15. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Ettinger U, Joober R, DeGuzman R, O’Driscoll GA. Schizotypy, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and dopamine genes. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2006) 60:764–7. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01594.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Schofield K, Mohr C. Schizotypy and hemispheric asymmetry: results from two Chapman scales, the O-LIFE questionnaire, and two laterality measures. Laterality (2014) 19:178–200. doi:10.1080/1357650X.2013.789883

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Mohr C, Hubener F, Laska M. Deviant olfactory experiences, magical ideation, and olfactory sensitivity: a study with healthy German and Japanese subjects. Psychiatry Res (2002) 111:21–33. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00132-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Taurisano P, Romano R, Mancini M, Giorgio AD, Antonucci LA, Fazio L, et al. Prefronto-striatal physiology is associated with schizotypy and is modulated by a functional variant of DRD2. Front Behav Neurosci (2014) 8:235. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00235

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Weinberger DR. Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci (2006) 7:818–27. doi:10.1038/nrn1993

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Gray NS, Pickering AD, Gray JA. Psychoticism and dopamine D2 binding in the basal ganlia using single photon emission tomography. Pers Indiv Differ (1994) 17:431–4. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90289-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Farde L, Gustavsson JP, Jonsson E. D2 dopamine receptors and personality traits. Nature (1997) 385:590. doi:10.1038/385590a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Breier A, Kestler L, Adler C, Elman I, Wiesenfeld N, Malhotra A, et al. Dopamine D2 receptor density and personal detachment in healthy subjects. Am J Psychiatry (1998) 155:1440–2.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Chen KC, Lee IH, Yeh TL, Chiu NT, Chen PS, Yang YK, et al. Schizotypy trait and striatal dopamine receptors in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Res (2012) 201:218–21. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Howes OD, Shotbolt P, Bloomfield M, Daalman K, Demjaha A, Diederen KM, et al. Dopaminergic function in the psychosis spectrum: an [18F]-DOPA imaging study in healthy individuals with auditory hallucinations. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39:807–14. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr195

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Breier A, Su TP, Saunders R, Carson RE, Kolachana BS, Bartolomeis A, et al. Schizophrenia is associated with elevated amphetamine-induced synaptic dopamine concentrations: evidence from a novel positron emission tomography method. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1997) 94:2569–74. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.6.2569

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Woodward ND, Cowan RL, Park S, Ansari MS, Baldwin RM, Li R, et al. Correlation of individual differences in schizotypal personality traits with amphetamine-induced dopamine release in striatal and extrastriatal brain regions. Am J Psychiatry (2011) 168:418–26. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10020165

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. van OJ, Kenis G, Rutten BP. The environment and schizophrenia. Nature (2010) 468:203–12. doi:10.1038/nature09563

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Soliman A, O’Driscoll GA, Pruessner J, Holahan AL, Boileau I, Gagnon D, et al. Stress-induced dopamine release in humans at risk of psychosis: a [11C]raclopride PET study. Neuropsychopharmacology (2008) 33:2033–41. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301597

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Gruzelier J. A Janusian perspective on the nature, development and structure of schizophrenia and schizotypy. Schizophr Res (2002) 54:95–103. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00356-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Deyoung CG. The neuromodulator of exploration: a unifying theory of the role of dopamine in personality. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:762. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00762

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Conrad K. Die Beginnende Schizophrenie. Versuch Einer Gestaltanalyse Des Wahns. Stuttgart: Thieme (1958).

Google Scholar

190. Brugger P, Regard M, Landis T, Cook N, Krebs D, Niederberger J. ‘Meaningful’ patterns in visual noise: effects of lateral stimulation and the observer’s belief in ESP. Psychopathology (1993) 26:261–5. doi:10.1159/000284831

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. van Elk M. Paranormal and religious believers are more prone to illusory face perception than skeptics and non-believers. Appl Cogn Psychol (2013) 27:150–5. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.07.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. Bressan P. The connection between random sequences, everyday coincidences, and belief in the paranormal. Appl Cogn Psychol (2002) 16:17–34. doi:10.1002/acp.754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

193. Brugger P. From haunted brain to haunted science: a cognitive neuroscience view of paranormal and pseudoscientific thought. In: Houran J, Lange R, editors. Hauntings and Poltergeists: Multidisciplinary Perspectives. McFarland (2001). p. 195–213.

Google Scholar

194. Shaner A. Delusions, superstitious conditioning and chaotic dopamine neurodynamics. Med Hypotheses (1999) 52:119–23. doi:10.1054/mehy.1997.0656

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

195. McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. J Pers (1992) 60:175–215. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

196. Winton-Brown TT, Fusar-Poli P, Ungless MA, Howes OD. Dopaminergic basis of salience dysregulation in psychosis. Trends Neurosci (2014) 37:85–94. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2013.11.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

197. Carpenter WT, Koenig JI. The evolution of drug development in schizophrenia: past issues and future opportunities. Neuropsychopharmacology (2008) 33:2061–79. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301639

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

198. Skosnik PD, Spatz-Glenn L, Park S. Cannabis use is associated with schizotypy and attentional disinhibition. Schizophr Res (2001) 48:83–92. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00132-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

199. Larrison AL, Briand KA, Sereno AB. Nicotine, caffeine, alcohol and schizotypy. Pers Indiv Differ (1999) 27:101–8. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00217-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

200. Williams JH, Wellman NA, Rawlins JN. Tobacco smoking correlates with schizotypal and borderline personality traits. Pers Indiv Differ (1996) 20:267–70. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(95)00179-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

201. Ripke S, O’Dushlaine C, Chambert K, Moran JL, Kähler AK, Akterin S, et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia. Nat Genet (2013) 45:1150–9. doi:10.1038/ng.2742

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizotypy, personality, dopamine, cognition, psychopharmacology, neuroimaging, genetics

Citation: Mohr C and Ettinger U (2014) An overview of the association between schizotypy and dopamine. Front. Psychiatry 5:184. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00184

Received: 11 May 2014; Accepted: 05 December 2014;
Published online: 19 December 2014.

Edited by:

Caroline Gurvich, Monash University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Ahmed A. Moustafa, University of Western Sydney, Australia
Maria R. Dauvermann, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA
Colin G. DeYoung, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, USA

Copyright: © 2014 Mohr and Ettinger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christine Mohr, Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Quartier Mouline, Bâtiment Géopolis, Lausanne 1015, Switzerland e-mail: christine.mohr@unil.ch;
Ulrich Ettinger, Department of Psychology, University of Bonn, Kaiser-Karl-Ring 9, Bonn 53111, Germany e-mail: ulrich.ettinger@uni-bonn.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.