Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Childhood trauma and cognitive biases associated with psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis

  • Jazz Croft ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    jazz.croft@bristol.ac.uk

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • David Martin,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • Paul Madley-Dowd,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • Daniela Strelchuk,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • Jonathan Davies,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • Jon Heron,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • Christoph Teufel,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre, School of Psychology, University of Cardiff, Cardiff, United Kingdom

  • Stanley Zammit

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom, Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Medical Research Council Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, United Kingdom

Abstract

Childhood trauma is associated with an increased risk of psychosis, but the mechanisms that mediate this relationship are unknown. Exposure to trauma has been hypothesised to lead to cognitive biases that might have causal effects on psychotic symptoms. The literature on whether childhood trauma is associated with psychosis-related cognitive biases has not been comprehensively reviewed. A systematic review and meta-analysis or narrative synthesis of studies examining the association between childhood trauma and the following biases: external locus of control (LOC), external attribution, probabilistic reasoning, source monitoring, top-down processing, and bias against disconfirmatory evidence. Studies were assessed for quality, and sources of heterogeneity were explored. We included 25 studies from 3,465 studies identified. Individuals exposed to childhood trauma reported a more external LOC (14 studies: SMD Median = 0.40, Interquartile range 0.07 to 0.52), consistent with a narrative synthesis of 11 other studies of LOC. There was substantial heterogeneity in the meta-analysis (I2 = 93%) not explained by study characteristics examined. Narrative syntheses for other biases showed weaker, or no evidence of association with trauma. The quality of included studies was generally low. Our review provides some evidence of an association between childhood trauma and a more external LOC, but not with the other biases examined. The low quality and paucity of studies for most of the cognitive biases examined highlights the need for more rigorous studies to determine which biases occur after trauma, and whether they mediate an effect of childhood trauma on psychosis.

Introduction

Psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia are a leading cause of disability and contribute to an increasing global disease burden [1, 2]. Exposure to abuse and neglect during childhood is associated with an increased likelihood of psychosis across the spectrum of symptom severity from sub-clinical psychotic experiences to psychotic disorder [3]. Meta-analyses estimate that the risk of psychosis is increased by 2–3 times [4] in those exposed to childhood trauma. However, there is a paucity of knowledge about the mechanisms that might explain how exposure to childhood trauma leads to the development of psychotic experiences. Current treatments for psychosis are of limited efficacy [5], and there is therefore a pressing need to understand more about the aetiology of these disorders to identify targets for preventative interventions.

Cognitive (information-processing) biases have been posited as a mechanism by which exposure to trauma can lead to the development of psychotic phenomena such as hallucinations and delusional beliefs, and can be broadly categorised as biases in causal attribution, interpretation, and inference. Causal attribution refers to how individuals interpret the outcome of events in terms of agency and responsibility. Previous studies have found that people with psychosis are more likely to believe that external forces (e.g. luck, fate) are accountable for the outcome of events (external locus of control [6, 7]) and to ascribe causality to situational factors (e.g. other people, institutional bias) for these events (external attribution bias [8]) compared to people without psychosis. Biases in the interpretation of new information that have been associated with psychosis include a bias for identifying internally-generated information (e.g. thoughts or speech) as coming from an external source (e.g. media or other people; source monitoring bias [9]), an over-reliance on prior knowledge over incoming stimuli when interpreting new information (top-down processing bias [10]), and a bias for interpreting neutral stimuli as threatening (attention to threat bias [11, 12]). Finally, biases in inference include hasty decision making in probabilistic inference tasks (the ‘Jumping to Conclusions’ bias; JTC [13]), and a resistance to revising beliefs in light of new information (a ‘Bias Against Disconfirmatory Evidence’; BADE [14]) have also been reported in people with psychotic symptoms.

Several theoretical models of psychosis have posited cognitive biases as candidate mechanisms on the pathway from exposure to trauma to subsequent psychotic symptoms [1518]. Perhaps the most comprehensive is the Bayesian Hierarchy model that integrates biological consequences of stress (such as dopaminergic and glutamatergic dysfunction) with cognitive neuroscience to describe how stressors could lead to biases in attribution, interpretation and inference through disruption in prediction error signalling, and how this can lead to the formation of both hallucinatory experiences and delusional beliefs [17, 19]. However, whilst the increased prevalence of cognitive biases in psychosis has been established and theoretical models of how trauma can lead to biases exist, it is not clear whether experiences of childhood trauma are associated with these same biases.

We sought to inform theoretical models of psychosis by systematically reviewing studies that have examined the association between exposure to childhood trauma and biases in cognition and perception. Our aim was to examine whether individuals who were, or were not, exposed to trauma before the age of 18 years differed in performance on tasks that assessed bias in the following domains: locus of control, attribution bias, source monitoring, probabilistic reasoning, bias against disconfirmatory evidence, and top-down processing.

Methods

The full search protocol was pre-registered in Prospero (ID: CRD42017059401).

Literature search

We (J.C.) searched the following databases: PsychInfo, OvidMedliner and PILOTs on 15th February 2020 using relevant key words and subject headings for exposure to different types of trauma and for each specific cognitive and perceptual task (for a complete list see Search Terms in S1 File).

Inclusion criteria

Inclusion criteria are detailed in the supplementary materials (see Protocol and Fig A in S1 File). Articles must have been published in a peer-reviewed journal, in English, and compared performance on cognitive or perceptual bias tasks (detailed below) between participants who reported exposure to trauma prior to 18 years of age and those who did not.

The biases included are described briefly below. It should be noted that whilst attention to threat biases are also commonly described in people with psychosis this bias was not included in our study as a review of research on trauma and attention to threat bias was recently published [20].

External locus of control.

A locus of control (LOC) refers to the extent to which an individual believes themselves to be accountable for their actions and is a specific dimension of attributional style [21, 22].

External attribution bias.

Attribution theory refers to the way in which an individual ascribes causality to events; either to personal qualities (internal) or to others or situational factors (external) [23, 24].

Source monitoring.

Source Monitoring refers to an individual’s ability to track actions and speech as produced by themselves or others and is also referred to as reality monitoring [9, 25].

Jumping to conclusions bias.

A ‘jumping to conclusions’ bias refers to individuals making judgements hastily based on limited information, which can lead to reaching unwarranted conclusions [13, 26].

Top-down processing bias.

An overreliance on prior expectations when perceiving new stimuli, also referred to as a greater influence of ‘top-down’ modulation in visual and auditory domains [27, 28]. This has been observed in visual and auditory domains [10, 29].

Bias against disconfirmatory evidence.

A bias against disconfirmatory evidence (BADE) refers to a bias against revising initial probability estimations when presented with additional evidence that may contradict an individual’s initial estimation [30, 31].

Study selection and data extraction

One author (JC) screened all abstracts and obtained full texts of papers that might potentially meet inclusion criteria. Working independently, two authors (J.C and one of P.M.D, D.S, or J.D) screened full-text articles to determine if they met inclusion criteria (see Fig A in S1 File). Data were extracted independently (J.C & D.M). Any discrepancies in decisions at any stage of the screening were resolved by a third reviewer.

Quality assessment

Internal validity was assessed by two authors (J.C & D.M) independently rated each study using a version of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale, a widely-used risk of bias tool for observational studies, adapted for this study by the study authors (see Fig B in S1 File). Studies were rated based on the presence or absence of the following criteria: i) Random or complete sampling (1 point), ii) Response rate of 75% or more (1 points), iii) Non-exposed sample representative of exposed sample (1 point), iv) Adjustment for confounders (max. 2 points), v) Observer bias minimised (1 point). Total scores were calculated by summing scores across these 5 criteria (possible score 0–6). We focused particularly on sampling strategy, observer bias, and adjustment for confounding as the most likely sources of biased results. The variables identified a priori as the most likely potential confounders were: sex, markers of cognitive functioning (e.g. IQ), socio-economic status and age.

Data analysis

A meta-analysis was only possible for studies that examined LOC. For each study, the sample n, mean LOC score and standard deviation of the exposed and non-exposed groups were used to derive a standardised mean difference (SMD) in LOC score. For studies that measured LOC in separate groups that were exposed to trauma, means and standard deviations were combined according to Cochrane guidelines [32]. A random effects meta-analysis of the SMD was conducted using the ‘metan’ [33] command in STATA version 15. Heterogeneity was assessed using the I2 statistic. Possible publication bias was assessed using an Egger’s regression test [34]. Meta-regression was used to assess whether likely sources of variation (study quality, recruitment sample, mean age, and sex distribution of sample) were associated with effect estimates and explained any heterogeneity. Where insufficient data were available to conduct a meta-analysis, studies were summarised using a narrative synthesis.

Results

Search results

The literature search resulted in a total of 4,144 references. After reading titles and abstracts of 3,906 de-duplicated results, 105 articles were reviewed in full and assessed for eligibility according to the inclusion criteria. After reading the full text, 79 full articles were excluded (PRISMA flow diagram, Fig 1).

Included studies

We included 26 studies [21, 3559] that fulfilled all search criteria (see Table 1 for summary of studies). The studies were based in the following countries: USA (12 studies) [21, 35, 38, 41, 43, 48, 5055], UK (3 studies) [39, 44, 46], Australia (2 studies) [40, 49], and 1 each in Canada [47], China [59], Greece [36], Holland [45], Italy [56], Japan [58], New Zealand [57], Turkey [37] and Taiwan [42]. Six studies [38, 40, 48, 51, 52, 56] recruited separate exposed and unexposed samples and the remaining studies were cross-sectional designs. Thirteen studies recruited participants who were less than 18 years of age [37, 38, 43, 4749, 51, 52, 55, 56, 58, 60], 12 recruited participants over 18 years of age [21, 35, 39, 40, 42, 4446, 50, 53, 54, 57, 59] and 1 study [41] sampled participants across this age threshold. With the exemption of two studies that relied on assessments from health professionals [38, 56], studies obtained exposure data using a range of self-report questionnaires relating to different types of trauma (See Table A in S1 File). Twenty-six studies examined only one type of cognitive or perceptual bias. One study [48] examined both LOC and external attribution bias, contributing two results to the review. No included studies examined a bias for relying on top-down knowledge to disambiguate new information, or a bias against disconfirmatory evidence.

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of studies included in the systematic review.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0246948.t001

Association between trauma and bias

Locus of control.

There were 20 studies that examined LOC. Nine different scales were used to assess LOC, the most common being the Nowicki-Strickland Scale for Children [61] (8 studies) [36, 37, 47, 48, 52, 55, 56, 58].. Of the 20 studies, 14 had data available to compare the SMD in LOC scores between those exposed and unexposed to childhood trauma using a meta-analysis.

A total of 12,691 participants were included in the meta-analysis from these 14 studies with a median of 155 participants (range 27 to 4351). An average of 41.2% (SD = 18.7) of the participants were exposed to trauma. Results for sub-groups of trauma exposure were combined in a pooled analysis for three studies [36, 55, 62]. One study [39] reported two results from the same sample—one for exposure to physical abuse and one for exposure to sexual abuse; to reduce bias in the pooled analysis, only the exposure with the higher prevalence (sexual abuse) was included.

The SMD suggested a greater (more external) LOC in the exposed group (SMD Median = 0.40, IQR 0.07 to 0.52). However, there was substantial heterogeneity between studies (I2 = 94.4%; Tau-squared = 0.15). As illustrated in Fig 2, one study [36] was a clear outlier in the analysis. When omitting this study, the I2 value reduced to 81.2% and Tau-Squared to .05.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Meta-analysis forest plot of childhood trauma and Standardised Mean Difference (SMD) in locus of control.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0246948.g002

A series of meta-regressions were carried out to examine possible sources of variation between studies. None of the variables we examined (study quality, recruitment sample, mean age, and sex distribution of sample) were associated with effect sizes across the included studies. The I2 value was reduced to the greatest extent by including the sex distribution of the sample (76.3%; see Table B in S1 File).

Five studies were not included in the meta-analysis as insufficient data were published and were not provided by the study authors [43, 47, 49, 51, 56] when requested. There was some evidence of an association between exposure to trauma and an external LOC reported by all five studies. Three studies examined forms of peer victimisation (correlation with a more external LOC ranging from r = .23 to r = .55; p-values < .05 to .003) [43, 47, 49] and two reported a relationship between maltreatment and a more external LOC [51, 56].

External attribution bias.

Two studies examined the association between childhood trauma and external attribution bias. One study [57] examined the external attribution bias in participants with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder but found no association with childhood trauma in either group. In the other study, sexually abused children reported a greater personal attribution to negative events compared to non-abused children (p < .05) [48].

Source monitoring.

Four studies examined performance on source monitoring tasks. Two of these did not report any evidence of an association between trauma exposure (62%-79% exposed) and source monitoring: one involved a sample of first-episode psychosis patients [40], and the other involved a sample of female, acute psychiatric patients [42]. In the third study, using a non-clinical sample, participants who reported exposure to sexual abuse had a lower sensitivity (d’) in distinguishing between real and imagined stimuli compared to non-exposed individuals (block 1: r = 0.12, p = .07; block 2: r = 0.19, p = .01) [50]. In the fourth study which was in a sample of university students, there was some evidence to suggest that participants exposed to trauma (29%) were more likely to misattribute externally-generated stimuli to self-generated sources in a hierarchical regression analysis after including confounders (B = -0.30, SE = 0.11, T = -2.86, p<0.01) [59].

Jumping to conclusions bias.

Only one study [44] tested for the presence of the JTC bias, in a sample of 200 members of the general population. Twenty percent of this sample demonstrated this bias, but there was no association with childhood trauma (OR = 1.1, 95% CI 0.44, 2.75; p = .831). It should be noted that the proportion of the sample exposed to childhood trauma was not reported.

Quality assessment

The assessment of quality for each paper is summarised in Table 2. Quality scores ranged from 2 to 6. Only six studies (23%) fulfilled over half of the criteria and the mean score across the 26 studies was 2.74 (SD = 1.02). The most poorly met criteria were related to sampling. Twenty-three studies (88%) did not use a random sample or sample a complete group, and 18 studies (69%) either had a low response rate (<75%) or failed to report a response rate. Of the 6 studies that described sampling from separate groups, 2 studies [63, 64] described sampling from the same community and were assessed as being representative of the exposed cohort. Of the remaining 4 studies, 2 [48, 65] reported a higher SES in the non-exposed group and adjusted for this in the analysis, and 2 [40, 65] did not provide details of whether the groups were from the same community.

Sixteen (62%) of the studies included in the review described procedures that aimed to minimise observer bias in assessing the outcome, most commonly through delivering self-report measures. Sixteen studies (62%) controlled for at least one variable that was identified by reviewers as a potentially important confounder. Only 3 studies (12%) controlled for multiple confounding variables. The results from the Egger’s regression provides weak evidence of an asymmetrical distribution in the funnel-plot of the meta-analysis. The estimated bias coefficient is 3.15 (95% CI = < .001, 7.03, p = 0.050; for funnel plot of distribution see Fig C in S1 File).

Discussion

This review provides a comprehensive summary of research that examines the relationship between exposure to trauma in childhood and cognitive and perceptual biases associated with psychosis. There was some evidence of a difference in performance on cognitive tasks between those exposed and not exposed to childhood trauma. However, this was not observed for all the cognitive and perceptual tasks included in our search criteria. With the exception of LOC, a very small number of studies assessed the external attribution, source monitoring and probabilistic reasoning biases and no studies examined a bias for relying on top-down knowledge to disambiguate new information, or a bias against disconfirmatory evidence.

Furthermore, there was substantial heterogeneity (defined as an I2 >75%) in the meta-analysis of measures of LOC, making the estimated pooled effect size for this measure difficult to interpret. Heterogeneity was reduced when a low-quality study that was a clear outlier [36] was omitted from the meta-analysis. However, there was minimal evidence that any of the study characteristics we examined could account for this heterogeneity, and it remained substantial. Nevertheless, the findings from the meta-analysis, supported by those from the narrative synthesis, are consistent with a more external LOC in participants exposed to trauma. Increased externality of an individual’s locus of control is associated with a range of negative mental health outcomes [66], and has been often described in people with psychosis [23]. An external LOC has also been shown to mediate part of the association between childhood trauma and psychotic experiences [7]. Explanations for how trauma can lead to causal attributions and a more external LOC include psychological (for example by generating feelings of inferiority [67] and undermining estimations of self-efficacy) and biological (for example through disruption of dopaminergic and glutamatergic pathways) ones. Indeed, current aetiological models of psychosis attempt to integrate epidemiological (including trauma), biological, psychological and cognitive findings to explain how psychotic symptoms develop [17, 68].

We also observed some evidence to suggest that children exposed to sexual abuse had a bias towards more external attribution in negative situations [48], although this was not observed in children exposed to multiple traumas in another study [64]. Further exploration of the role of trauma type was limited by the small number of studies that examined this. Findings for source monitoring deficits and the JTC bias were also limited by the small number of studies examining these. Furthermore, we found no studies that examined exposure to trauma and top-down processing biases or bias against disconfirmatory evidence.

The quality assessment of studies highlighted potential sources of bias in a large proportion of the studies: only eight of the studies included (30%) satisfied more than half of the quality assessment criteria. A small proportion of studies (n = 8; 30%) reported a response rate of 75% or more, with even fewer studies (n = 4; 15%) reporting random or complete sampling, raising the likelihood that the findings from this review are influenced by selection bias. The role of confounding variables was also considered in the quality assessment. Sixteen (62%) of the studies adjusted for confounders in their analysis, but only one study [69] provided information on both unadjusted and adjusted results, meaning the extent to which confounding explains the association between childhood trauma and cognitive biases remains unclear.

A number of studies not included in this review as they met some, but not all of the inclusion criteria are summarised in Table B in S1 File. These studies reported a relationship between trauma and a more external LOC, which is consistent with the findings of this review. However, the one study that examined the JTC bias and external attribution bias did not find evidence of an association with trauma [70].

Strengths and limitations

The review’s research questions benefit from an established theoretical framework based on the premise that cognitive biases are a mechanism on the causal pathway between childhood trauma and psychotic symptoms. A methodological strength was that we followed PRISMA guidelines throughout the review (see S2 File). The meta-analysis was based on data from a large number of participants and the majority of studies that tested LOC used scales that could be standardised for use in a pooled analysis. We were unable to include all studies in the meta-analysis because some studies did not provide the required data, either within the paper or on request. The review was limited by its restriction to English-language and peer-reviewed publications, so we may have missed studies that could have contributed to addressing our study aims.

As summarised in the supporting information (Table A in S1 File), there was a diverse range of measures used to assess trauma, including referrals from social services and various self-report measures, and this may have contributed to the wide range of exposure prevalence and the variation in results across these studies. Measurement error could also vary across measures as childhood trauma data collected by questionnaire may be less reliable compared to data collected by interview [71]. In addition, few studies distinguished between witnessed and experienced trauma, or provided information on other indicators of trauma severity that may have also contributed to heterogeneity in our findings. Furthermore, while we were able to test if the average age of the sample was a predictor of effect size, we were unable to examine whether the time since trauma exposure (recency effect) contributed to heterogeneity. Finally, while most studies measured LOC using well-established measures, including the Nowicki-Strickland and Rotter scales, studies that used less widely implemented scales may have increased variation in results across studies.

Implications

Our study shows that there are very few studies that examine the relationship between exposure to childhood trauma and cognitive and perceptual biases associated with psychosis. The exceptions to this are LOC as reviewed here and attention to threat bias, as reviewed separately [20]. Results from analyses of these biases seem to support an association with trauma, though confidence that these associations are causal rather than due to confounding or bias is low. Based on these results, there is a clear need for future studies to employ more rigorous methodology and to examine the role of confounding more thoroughly. For example, markers of adverse family environments (such as poverty, parental substance use, and parental psychiatric disorder), as well as child-related factors such as temperament and cognitive ability could all be associated with risk of childhood trauma exposure and also affect the way a child perceives and makes inferences about the world around them. Clearer reporting of descriptive statistics and results of any tests of association, including both unadjusted and adjusted estimates, would also make it easier to combine estimates in future meta-analyses.

Our review highlights an important evidence gap in our understanding of potential mechanisms by which a greater risk of psychosis might arise in those exposed to childhood trauma. Further study is needed to ascertain whether the aforementioned information-processing biases occur as a result of trauma and the extent to which they mediate the relationship between trauma and psychosis. This might then help to identify those biases most likely to offer potential targets for the development of new therapeutic interventions for psychotic symptoms.

Conclusions

Our review provides some evidence of an association between exposure to childhood trauma and a more external LOC, but not for the other cognitive biases examined here. Whilst an external LOC might be a candidate mechanism that mediates an effect of childhood trauma on psychosis, most studies that examined this relationship were of low quality, and further studies are required to strengthen the currently weak evidence that this bias arises as a consequence of trauma.

Supporting information

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Julian Higgins and the Jean Goulding Institute for providing advice regarding data analysis.

References

  1. 1. Charlson FJ, Ferrari AJ, Santomauro DF, Diminic S, Stockings E, Scott JG, et al. Global Epidemiology and Burden of Schizophrenia: Findings From the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Schizophr Bull [Internet]. 2018 [cited 2018 Jul 25]; Available from: https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/advance-article/doi/10.1093/schbul/sby058/4995547 pmid:29762765
  2. 2. Ferrari AJ, Stockings E, Khoo J-P, Erskine HE, Degenhardt L, Vos T, et al. The prevalence and burden of bipolar disorder: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Bipolar Disord. 2016;18:440–50. pmid:27566286
  3. 3. Bendall S, Jackson HJ, Hulbert CA, McGorry PD. Childhood Trauma and Psychotic Disorders: a Systematic, Critical Review of the Evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:568–79. pmid:18003630
  4. 4. Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, Lieverse R, Lataster T, Viechtbauer W, et al. Childhood Adversities Increase the Risk of Psychosis: A Meta-analysis of Patient-Control, Prospective- and Cross-sectional Cohort Studies. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:661–71. pmid:22461484
  5. 5. Patel KR, Cherian J, Gohil K, Atkinson D. Schizophrenia: Overview and Treatment Options. Pharm Ther. 2014;39:638–45. pmid:25210417
  6. 6. Sullivan S, Bentall RP, Fernyhough C, Pearson RM, Zammit S. Cognitive Styles and Psychotic Experiences in a Community Sample. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e80055. pmid:24244608
  7. 7. Fisher HL, Schreier A, Zammit S, Maughan B, Munafò MR, Lewis G, et al. Pathways Between Childhood Victimization and Psychosis-like Symptoms in the ALSPAC Birth Cohort. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:1045–55. pmid:22941743
  8. 8. Randjbar S, Veckenstedt R, Vitzthum F, Hottenrott B, Moritz S. Attributional biases in paranoid schizophrenia: Further evidence for a decreased sense of self‐causation in paranoia. Psychosis. 2011;3:74–85.
  9. 9. Garrison JR, Bond R, Gibbard E, Johnson MK, Simons JS. Monitoring what is real: The effects of modality and action on accuracy and type of reality monitoring error. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav. 2017;87:108–17. pmid:27444616
  10. 10. Teufel C, Subramaniam N, Dobler V, Perez J, Finnemann J, Mehta PR, et al. Shift toward prior knowledge confers a perceptual advantage in early psychosis and psychosis-prone healthy individuals. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112:13401–6. pmid:26460044
  11. 11. Hillmann TE, Kempkensteffen J, Lincoln TM. Visual Attention to Threat-Related Faces and Delusion-Proneness: An Eye Tracking Study Using Dynamic Stimuli. Cogn Ther Res. 2015;39:808–15.
  12. 12. Lim M, Gleeson JF, Jackson HJ. Selective Attention to Threat Bias in Delusion-Prone Individuals: J Nerv Ment Dis. 2011;199:765–72. pmid:21964270
  13. 13. Dudley R, Taylor P, Wickham S, Hutton P. Psychosis, delusions and the “jumping to conclusions” reasoning bias: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2015;sbv150. pmid:26519952
  14. 14. Woodward TS, Moritz S, Menon M, Klinge R. Belief inflexibility in schizophrenia. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:267–77.
  15. 15. Garety , Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D, Bebbington PE. A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychol Med. 2001;31:189–95. pmid:11232907
  16. 16. Bentall RP, Fernyhough C. Social Predictors of Psychotic Experiences: Specificity and Psychological Mechanisms. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:1012–20. pmid:18703667
  17. 17. Howes OD, Kapur S. The Dopamine Hypothesis of Schizophrenia: Version III—The Final Common Pathway. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:549–62. pmid:19325164
  18. 18. Broyd A, Balzan RP, Woodward TS, Allen P. Dopamine, cognitive biases and assessment of certainty: A neurocognitive model of delusions. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;54:96–106. pmid:28448827
  19. 19. Corlett PR, Frith CD, Fletcher PC. From drugs to deprivation: a Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009;206:515–30.
  20. 20. da Silva Ferreira GC, Crippa JAS, de Lima Osório F. Facial emotion processing and recognition among maltreated children: a systematic literature review. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2014 [cited 2018 May 21];5. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4269127/ pmid:25566138
  21. 21. Asberg K, Renk K. Perceived Stress, External Locus of Control, and Social Support as Predictors of Psychological Adjustment Among Female Inmates With or Without a History of Sexual Abuse. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2014;58:59–84. pmid:23070955
  22. 22. Sullivan SA, Thompson A, Kounali D, Lewis G, Zammit S. The longitudinal association between external locus of control, social cognition and adolescent psychopathology. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017;52:643–55. pmid:28271211
  23. 23. Thompson A, Papas A, Bartholomeusz C, Nelson B, Yung A. Externalized attributional bias in the Ultra High Risk (UHR) for psychosis population. Psychiatry Res. 2013;206:200–5. pmid:23177592
  24. 24. Lincoln TM, Mehl S, Exner C, Lindenmeyer J, Rief W. Attributional Style and Persecutory Delusions. Evidence for an Event Independent and State Specific External-Personal Attribution Bias for Social Situations. Cogn Ther Res. 2010;34:297–302.
  25. 25. Griffin JD, Fletcher PC. Predictive Processing, Source Monitoring, and Psychosis. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13:265–89. pmid:28375719
  26. 26. Garety P, Freeman D, Jolley S, Ross K, Waller H, Dunn G. Jumping to conclusions: the psychology of delusional reasoning. Adv Psychiatr Treat. 2011;17:332–9.
  27. 27. Cook J, Barbalat G, Blakemore S-J. Top-down modulation of the perception of other people in schizophrenia and autism. Front Hum Neurosci [Internet]. 2012;6. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3375615/
  28. 28. Aleman A, Böcker KBE, Hijman R, de Haan EHF, Kahn RS. Cognitive basis of hallucinations in schizophrenia: role of top-down information processing. Schizophr Res. 2003;64:175–85. pmid:14613682
  29. 29. Daalman K, Verkooijen S, Derks EM, Aleman A, Sommer IEC. The influence of semantic top-down processing in auditory verbal hallucinations. Schizophr Res. 2012;139:82–6. pmid:22727455
  30. 30. Bronstein MV, Cannon TD. Measuring bias against disconfirmatory evidence: An evaluation of BADE task scoring methods and the case for a novel method. Psychiatry Res. 2018;261:535–40. pmid:29407719
  31. 31. Eisenacher S, Rausch F, Mier D, Fenske S, Veckenstedt R, Englisch S, et al. Bias against disconfirmatory evidence in the ‘at-risk mental state’ and during psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2016;238:242–50. pmid:27086240
  32. 32. Higgins JPT, Green S, Cochrane Collaboration. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions [Internet]. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell; 2008 [cited 2018 Oct 29]. Available from: https://www.dawsonera.com/guard/protected/dawson.jsp?name = https://idp.bris.ac.uk/shibboleth&dest = http://www.dawsonera.com/depp/reader/protected/external/AbstractView/S9780470712177
  33. 33. Harris R, Bradburn M, Deeks J, Harbord RM, Altman DG, Sterne J. metan: fixed- and random-effects meta-analysis. Stata J. 2008;8:3–28.
  34. 34. Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315:629–34. pmid:9310563
  35. 35. Allen B, Lauterbach D. Personality characteristics of adult survivors of childhood trauma. J Trauma Stress. 2017;20:587–95.
  36. 36. Andreou E. Bully/victim problems and their association with psychological constructs in 8- to 12-year-old Greek schoolchildren. Spec Issue Bullying Sch. 2000;26:49–56.
  37. 37. Atik G, Guneri OY. Bullying and victimization: Predictive role of individual, parental, and academic factors. Sch Psychol Int. 2013;34:658–73.
  38. 38. Barahal RM, Waterman J, Martin HP. The social cognitive development of abused children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1981;49:508–16. pmid:7264033
  39. 39. Beck-sander A. Childhood Abuse in Adult Offenders: The Role of Control in Perpetuating Cycles of Abuse. J Forensic Psychiatry. 1995;6:486–98.
  40. 40. Bendall S, Jackson HJ, Hulbert CA. What self-generated speech is externally misattributed in psychosis? Testing three cognitive models in a first-episode sample. Schizophr Res. 2011;129:36–41. pmid:21498051
  41. 41. Bolstad BR, Zinbarg RE. Sexual victimization, generalized perception of control, and posttraumatic stress disorder symptom severity. J Anxiety Disord. 1997;11:523–40. pmid:9407271
  42. 42. Chiu C-D, Tseng M-CM, Chien Y-L, Liao S-C, Liu C-M, Yeh Y-Y, et al. Misattributing the Source of Self-Generated Representations Related to Dissociative and Psychotic Symptoms. Front Psychol. 2016;7:541. pmid:27148147
  43. 43. Fredstrom BK, Adams RE, Gilman R. Electronic and school-based victimization: unique contexts for adjustment difficulties during adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2011;40:405–15. pmid:20628795
  44. 44. Freeman D, Pugh K, Garety P. Jumping to conclusions and paranoid ideation in the general population. Schizophr Res. 2008;102:254–60. pmid:18442898
  45. 45. Hovens JGFM, Giltay EJ, van Hemert AM, Penninx BWJH. Childhood Maltreatment and the Course of Depressive and Anxiety Disorders: The Contribution of Personality Characteristics. Depress Anxiety. 2016;33:27–34. pmid:26418232
  46. 46. Ireland CA, Alderson K, Ireland JL. Sexual Exploitation in Children: Nature, Prevalence, and Distinguishing Characteristics Reported in Young Adulthood. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma. 2015;24:603–22.
  47. 47. Luciano S, Savage RS. Bullying risk in children with learning difficulties in inclusive educational settings. Can J Sch Psychol. 2007;22:14–31.
  48. 48. Mannarino AP, Cohen JA. Abuse-Related Attributions and Perceptions, General Attributions, and Locus of Control in Sexually Abused Girls, Abuse-Related Attributions and Perceptions, General Attributions, and Locus of Control in Sexually Abused Girls. J Interpers Violence. 1996;11:162–80.
  49. 49. Marsh HW, Nagengast B, Morin A, J. S. Re, Parada RH, Craven RG, et al. Construct validity of the multidimensional structure of bullying and victimization: An application of exploratory structural equation modeling. J Educ Psychol. 2011;103:701–32.
  50. 50. McNally RJ, Clancy SA, Barrett HM, Parker HA. Reality monitoring in adults reporting repressed, recovered, or continuous memories of childhood sexual abuse. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:147–52. pmid:15709821
  51. 51. Moran PB, Eckenrode J. Protective personality characteristics among adolescent victims of maltreatment. Child Abuse Negl. 1992;16:743–54. pmid:1393733
  52. 52. Moyer DM, DiPietro L, Berkowitz RI, Stunkard AJ. Childhood sexual abuse and precursors of binge eating in an adolescent female population. Int J Eat Disord. 1997;21:23–30. pmid:8986514
  53. 53. Muller RD, Skues JL, Wise LZ. Cyberbullying in Australian primary schools: How victims differ in attachment, locus of control, self-esteem, and coping styles compared to non-victims. J Psychol Couns Sch. 2017;27:85–104.
  54. 54. Porter CA, Long PJ. Locus of control and adjustment in female adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse. J Child Sex Abuse Res Treat Program Innov Vict Surviv Offenders. 1999;8:3–25.
  55. 55. Radliff KM, Wang C, Swearer SM. Bullying and Peer Victimization: An Examination of Cognitive and Psychosocial Constructs. J Interpers Violence. 2016;31:1983–2005. pmid:25711618
  56. 56. Roazzi A, Attili G, Pentima LD, Toni A. Locus of control in maltreated children: the impact of attachment and cumulative trauma. Psicol Reflex E Crítica. 2016;29:8.
  57. 57. Rucklidge JJ, Brown DL, Crawford S, Kaplan BJ. Retrospective reports of childhood trauma in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2016;9:631–41.
  58. 58. Yamasaki S, Ando S, Koike S, Usami S, Endo K, French P, et al. Dissociation mediates the relationship between peer victimization and hallucinatory experiences among early adolescents. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2016;4:18–23. pmid:28740809
  59. 59. Chiu C-D, Ho HL, Tollenaar MS, Elzinga BM, Zhang T. Early relational trauma and self representations: Misattributing externally derived representations as internally generated. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. 2019;11:64–72. pmid:29697997
  60. 60. Betts LR, Houston JE, Steer OL, Gardner SE. Adolescents’ Experiences of Victimization: The Role of Attribution Style and Generalized Trust. J Sch Violence. Routledge; 2017;16:25–48.
  61. 61. Nowicki S, Strickland BR. A locus of control scale for children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1973;40:148–54.
  62. 62. Hovens JGFM, Giltay EJ, van Hemert AM, Penninx BWJH. CHILDHOOD MALTREATMENT AND THE COURSE OF DEPRESSIVE AND ANXIETY DISORDERS: THE CONTRIBUTION OF PERSONALITY CHARACTERISTICS. Depress Anxiety. 2016;33:27–34. pmid:26418232
  63. 63. Barahal RM, Waterman J, Martin HP. The social cognitive development of abused children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1981;49:508–16. pmid:7264033
  64. 64. Rucklidge JJ, Brown DL, Crawford S, Kaplan BJ. Retrospective reports of childhood trauma in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2016;9:631–41.
  65. 65. Moyer DM, DiPietro L, Berkowitz RI, Stunkard AJ. Childhood sexual abuse and precursors of binge eating in an adolescent female population. Int J Eat Disord. 1997;21:23–30. pmid:8986514
  66. 66. Frenkel E, Kugelmass S, Nathan M, Ingraham LJ. Locus of Control and Mental Health in Adolescence and Adulthood. Schizophr Bull. 1995;21:219–26. pmid:7631169
  67. 67. Bentall RP, Kinderman P, Kaney S. The self, attributional processes and abnormal beliefs: Towards a model of persecutory delusions. Behav Res Ther. 1994;32:331–41. pmid:8192633
  68. 68. Fletcher PC, Frith CD. Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:48–58. pmid:19050712
  69. 69. Barahal RM, Waterman J, Martin HP. The social cognitive development of abused children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1981;49:508–16. pmid:7264033
  70. 70. Gawęda Ł, Prochwicz K, Adamczyk P, Frydecka D, Misiak B, Kotowicz K, et al. The role of self-disturbances and cognitive biases in the relationship between traumatic life events and psychosis proneness in a non-clinical sample. Schizophr Res [Internet]. 2017 [cited 2018 Jan 23]; Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0920996417304243
  71. 71. Baldwin JR, Reuben A, Newbury JB, Danese A. Agreement Between Prospective and Retrospective Measures of Childhood Maltreatment: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2019 Mar 20]; Available from: https://jamanetwork.com/journals/jamapsychiatry/fullarticle/2728182 pmid:30892562