Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Investigation of the effect of cochlear implant electrode length on speech comprehension in quiet and noise compared with the results with users of electro-acoustic-stimulation, a retrospective analysis

  • Andreas Büchner ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Andreas Büchner, Angelika Illg

    Affiliation Medical University Hannover, Dept. of Otorhinolaryngology, Germany

  • Angelika Illg ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Andreas Büchner, Angelika Illg

    illg@hoerzentrum-hannover.de

    Affiliation Medical University Hannover, Dept. of Otorhinolaryngology, Germany

  • Omid Majdani,

    Affiliation Medical University Hannover, Dept. of Otorhinolaryngology, Germany

  • Thomas Lenarz

    Affiliation Medical University Hannover, Dept. of Otorhinolaryngology, Germany

Abstract

Objectives

This investigation evaluated the effect of cochlear implant (CI) electrode length on speech comprehension in quiet and noise and compare the results with those of EAS users.

Methodes

91 adults with some degree of residual hearing were implanted with a FLEX20, FLEX24, or FLEX28 electrode. Some subjects were postoperative electric-acoustic-stimulation (EAS) users; the other subjects were in the groups of electric stimulation-only (ES-only).

Speech perception was tested in quiet and noise at 3 and 6 months of ES or EAS use. Speech comprehension results were analyzed and correlated to electrode length.

Results

While the FLEX20 ES and FLEX24 ES groups were still in their learning phase between the 3 to 6 months interval, the FLEX28 ES group was already reaching a performance plateau at the three months appointment yielding remarkably high test scores. EAS subjects using FLEX20 or FLEX24 electrodes outscored ES-only subjects with the same short electrodes on all 3 tests at each interval, reaching significance with FLEX20 ES and FLEX24 ES subjects on all 3 tests at the 3-months interval and on 2 tests at the 6- months interval. Amongst ES-only subjects at the 3- months interval, FLEX28 ES subjects significantly outscored FLEX20 ES subjects on all 3 tests and the FLEX24 ES subjects on 2 tests. At the-6 months interval, FLEX28 ES subjects still exceeded the other ES-only subjects although the difference did not reach significance.

Conclusions

Among ES-only users, the FLEX28 ES users had the best speech comprehension scores, at the 3- months appointment and tendentially at the 6 months appointment. EAS users showed significantly better speech comprehension results compared to ES-only users with the same short electrodes.

Introduction

Many people with severe or severe-profound hearing loss in the high frequencies have functional residual hearing in the low frequencies. For such people, electric-acoustic-stimulation (EAS) or hybrid systems, which combine the use of CI (electric stimulation, ES) and a hearing aid (acoustic stimulation) into one device, are advisable and can significantly benefit users, especially in difficult listening environments [1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6]. For EAS candidates and for CI candidates with less residual hearing, CI manufacturers developed thin and straight electrodes with lengths between 16 mm and 31 mm. The aim of these developments was to preserve residual hearing, even when it is marginal, by minimizing intraoperative damage to the sensitive intracochlear structures; while at the same time offering good speech understanding with electrical hearing only. If a CI recipient does lose residual hearing due to surgery, his/her electrically stimulated hearing must be superior to his/her preoperative speech understanding results or he/she will not have derived benefit from implantation.

With electrodes of different lengths, different insertion depths can be achieved. Insertion depth is determined by both the length of the selected electrode and the length of each individual candidate’s cochlear duct. Studies have shown the impact of cochlea size on insertion depth [7, 8, 9, 10]. In [11] a correlation between the preservation rate of residual hearing and corresponding insertion angle of the electrode was found whereas further studies demonstrated that hearing preservation is also possible with long electrodes [12,13,14,15]. The insertion depth angles differ. The FLEX electrode array in 10 temporal bone specimens vary between 341°±22° for FLEX20, 477°±36° for FLEX24, and 587°±42° for FLEX28 [8], indicating significant differences between the electrode arrays with respect to the cochlear coverage.

The influence of insertion depth on performance with electrical stimulation was shown by Hamzavi & Arnoldner [16] and by Buchman et al. [17]. Although their results showed that not all candidates benefited from the maximum number of activated channels, Hamzavi & Arnoldner [16] recommend deep insertion in candidates with profound hearing loss. Buchman et al. [17] compared a STANDARD (31 mm) with a MEDIUM (24 mm) electrode array in a prospective randomized study with 7 subjects receiving the long and 6 subjects receiving the shorter electrode array and found that deeper electrode insertion and greater insertion angles appear to offer better speech perception performance in the early post-activation period. It is interesting to mention that an interim analysis of the data in the latter study led to a discontinuation of the subject enrollment by the institutional review board because of the trend for better performance and faster improvement in the long electrode group. Further studies have found a positive correlation between the insertion depth and CI users’ speech perception [18, 19, 20, 21].

Other researchers found no positive or even negative correlation between insertion depth and speech understanding [22, 23, 24, 25, 26] with the caveat, however, that many different devices, electrode types and surgical techniques have been investigated and compared in these studies, introducing a high level of variability and complexity.

The aim of the present study was to analyze the influence of insertion depth and electrode length on the speech comprehension of ES-only and EAS users after 3- and 6-months of device use. The present study, with its comparatively large number of subjects, indicates that electrode length does play an important role in the ES-only user’s ability to hear in difficult listening environments. Almost all subjects evaluated in this study had residual acoustic hearing to some extent.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

The Ethics committee of the Medical University of Hannover, Germany, approved this retrospective study. No written information was given to patients because of the retrospective design. All patient data were anonymized and de-identified prior the retrospective analysis.

Subjects

The speech comprehension data of 91 cochlear implant patients were retrospectively analyzed after 3 and 6 months of device use. All subjects were postlingually hearing-impaired adult CI users who were implanted with a SONATA or CONCERTO with a FLEX20, FLEX24, or FLEX28 (MED-EL, Innsbruck, Austria) electrode array. Eighty five subjects were unilaterally implanted with FLEX electrodes. Sixty four of them used hearing aids on the contralateral side, 11 subjects were bilaterally implanted with standard electrodes by different CI companies on the contralateral side and 10 subjects used the implant unilaterally. Six subjects of this study were bilaterally implanted with FLEX electrodes, yielding a dataset of 97 ‘ears’. See Table 1 for subject demographics.

Etiology of deafness was unknown in 49 subjects, sudden hearing loss in 22 subjects, genetic in 7 subjects, viral in 4 subjects, noise-induced in 2 subjects, and 1 for each of the following: brainstem infarct, LVA-Syndrome, presbyacusis, otitis media, oxygen deficiency during birth, rubella embryopathy, and stapedectomy.

Subjects were divided into 4 groups depending on the length of their FLEX electrode and if they were an EAS or ES-only user. Group A consisted of EAS users with a FLEX20 or FLEX24 electrode; group B consisted of CI-only users with a FLEX20 electrode; group C consisted of CI-only users with a FLEX24 electrode; group D consisted of CI-only users with a FLEX28 electrode (Table 2). One subject switched from the EAS group (FLEX24 EAS) to the FLEX24 ES-only group after 3 months because he/she had insufficient residual hearing to benefit from continued acoustic amplification. Most subjects had some degree of pre- and postoperative residual hearing (Fig 1). In the subgroup of six patients which were bilaterally included in the study four patients were assigned with both ears to the same group: three patients in group A (FLEX20 EAS and FLEX24 EAS) and one in group D (FLEX28 ES). For one patient one ear was assigned to group C (FLEX24 ES) and the other ear in group D and for one patient one ear was assigned to group A and the other ear to group C and the patient switched to 6—months with both ears to group C. As the ears of the bilaterally included patients were tested independently, their results were treated as independent measurements.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Pre- and postoperative median hearing levels, minimum and maximum for all 97 ears, divided into groups: A = preoperatively, B = postoperatively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174900.g001

Patients were assigned to the electrode array based on their pre-operatively residual hearing. If patients had significant residual hearing in the low frequencies, they could choose to receive short electrode types, i.e. FLEX20 or FLEX 24, because of the possibility to use EAS postoperatively.

The postoperative residual hearing was measured about 6 weeks after implantation during the first fitting phase. For documentation of the residual hearing, the air-conduction hearing loss was measured at single frequencies between 125 Hz and 8 kHz. If no hearing could be measured up to the measurement limit of our audiometer, the threshold was set to audiometer limit + 5 dB. This is a best case assumption and corresponds to the audiometer limit + the minimum step size of the audiometer. The option for an electric-acoustic simulation was offered to all patients with a residual hearing of at least 75 dB at 250 Hz at the time of first fitting. If subjects had less residual hearing or did decide not to use acoustic amplification they were allocated to one of the ES-user groups (B, C, or D). The subjects in groups B, C, and D (ES user) had a variety of reasons for not using any acoustic amplification in everyday life (i.e. not being EAS users). Some had insufficient residual hearing postoperatively to use acoustic amplification, one did not like having the ear mold in his/her auricle, and one complained about tinnitus, others had no subjective benefit when wearing the acoustic stimulation. Some FLEX20 and FLEX24 subjects were not typical EAS-candidates, but asked for full preservation of their residual hearing, even though it was very little. For that reason, these subjects received either the FLEX20 or FLEX24 electrode—depending on how precious their residual hearing was to them, with the assumption that the shorter array gives better preservation than the longer one.

Fitting

In groups (B-D) with ES-only stimulation, 80 of the 82 ears used fine structure strategies FSP (fine structure processing) or FS4 since the time of first fitting. Only one patient of group D (bilaterally included in this study) was fitted with HDCIS (high-definition continuous interleaved sampling).

Both strategies are based on the CIS (continuous interleaved sampling) strategy described in [27]. The CIS strategy extracts the envelope information of each frequency band and provides a proportional stimulation current to the electrode contacts. The most recent CIS variant implemented in the MED-EL system is called HDCIS. With the fine structure strategies, additional temporal information is provided in the low frequency channels. This is done by dynamically adapting the stimulation rate on the most apical channels depending on the incoming acoustic signal [28]. The older implementation FSP offers rate pitch information on up to three fine structure channels, whereas FS4 guarantees fine structure information on the four most apical channels always.

In the group (A) with electric-acoustic-stimulation, 14 of 15 ears used fine structure strategies FSP or FS4 since their first activation. Only one patient (unilaterally included in this study) was fitted with HDCIS.

The low-frequency boundaries were primarily set based on the patients’ postoperative residual hearing and the patients’ subjective feedback.

Initially, the crossover-frequency between electric and acoustic hearing was set at the audiometric frequency where the hearing loss exceeds 65 to 80 dB HL. Based on patient’s feedback it was further adjusted and the preferred low-frequency boundary chosen. The resulting low-frequency cut-offs are between 100 Hz and 600 Hz.

Speech comprehension testing

After 3 and 6 months of device use, all subjects were tested using the Freiburg monosyllabic word test (FMT) [29] in quiet and the German language Hochmair-Desoyer, Schulz, Moser sentence test (HSM test) in quiet and in noise at 0° azimuth (S0N0) at a 10 dB signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) [30]. All tests were performed in free field with sentences and monosyllables presented at 65 dB SPL. The tests were conducted in the subjects’ everyday life condition with regard to their ipsilateral ear, meaning that the subjects in group A were tested in EAS-mode and with a closed contralateral ear.

The subjects in group B, C, and D were tested with open ipsilateral ear.

To avoid bilateral benefit, the CI or hearing aid of the contralateral side was turned off and taken away. Additionally, the contralateral ear was plugged in bimodal patients or bilateral EAS patients to eliminate the influence of the residual hearing.

Not every patient could complete each of the tests and subsequently, the number of subjects varied between tests. As a consequence, each figure contains the related number of patients for each condition.

Statistical analysis

Because the data were not normally distributed, the Mann-Whitney U Test for independent samples was used to evaluate hearing thresholds, demographic factors and speech comprehension data between the 4 groups. To compare the speech data gathered at the 3 and 6 months intervals, the Wilcoxon signed-rank test for dependent samples was used. For this comparison, patients’ data were excluded if only one of the two data sets (i.e. 3 or 6 months) was available. To investigate if demographic factors contribute to the speech data results, a hierarchical multiple linear regression analysis was performed. Dummy variables were defined in order to include the group variables (group A- group D) as categorical predictors into the model. The probable confounding factors age of implantation and duration of hearing impairment were added as predictors to the model in a stepwise manner. Based on the distribution of the predictors vs. dependent variable for all predictor variables a linear model was assumed without transformation. In order to check the assumption of linearity and heteroscedasticity the standardized residuals were checked for normal-distribution and for random distribution along the standardized predicted values. Statistical significance was set to p<0.05 (*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001). All data were analyzed statistically using IBM SPSS Statistics, 22.

Results

Hearing thresholds

The preoperative residual hearing in the low-frequencies (125 Hz– 1000 Hz) in patients receiving FLEX20 and FLEX24 electrodes was significantly better than for patients who received a longer FLEX28 electrode (FLEX20 to FLEX28: Mann–Whitney U = 187.5, n1 = 36, n2 = 35, p<0.001, FLEX24 to FLEX28: Mann–Whitney U = 205.5, n1 = 16, n2 = 35, p<0.001). Comparing the air-conduction hearing levels along the groups, patients with FLEX28 ES showed the poorest mean scores in the low frequencies PTA (125 Hz– 1000 Hz) preoperatively (Table 2). Significant differences were found between the residual hearing in the low frequencies of the EAS group (group A) compared to all single ES groups preoperatively (group A to group B: Mann–Whitney U = 77.5, n1 = 15, n2 = 23, p = 0.005, group A to group C: Mann–Whitney U = 62.5, n1 = 15, n2 = 24, p = 0.001, group A to group D: Mann–Whitney U = 16.5, n1 = 15, n2 = 35, p<0.001). Between FLEX20 ES and FLEX24 ES no significant differences were found (pre-op: Mann–Whitney U = 274, n1 = 23, n2 = 24, p = 0.966) in residual hearing.

Postoperatively, significant differences were found between the residual hearing in the low frequencies of the EAS group (A) compared to all ES groups (B-D) (group A to group B: Mann–Whitney U = 45, n1 = 15, n2 = 23, p<0.001, group A to group C: Mann–Whitney U = 67, n1 = 15, n2 = 21, p = 0.004, group A to group D: Mann–Whitney U = 14, n1 = 15, n2 = 35, p<0.001). Between FLEX20 ES (group B) and FLEX24 ES subjects (group C), no significant differences were found (post-op: Mann–Whitney U = 182.5, n1 = 23, n2 = 21, p = 0.165) in residual hearing. Group A (FLEX20 EAS and FLEX24 EAS) showed the best PTA (125 Hz– 1000 Hz) postoperatively (Table 2).

Demographic factors

In the demographics (Table 1) no significant difference were found in age of implantation and duration of hearing impairment between the groups. In duration of deafness a significant difference was found between group D compared to all other groups (group A to group D: Mann–Whitney U = 157.5, n1 = 15, n2 = 35, p = 0.05, group B to group D: Mann–Whitney U = 274, n1 = 23, n2 = 35, p = 0.09, group C to group D: Mann–Whitney U = 283.5, n1 = 24, n2 = 35, p = 0.07), because patients who had no residual hearing pre-operatively received a longer FLEX28 electrode and were assigned to group D, whereas in the groups A to C the duration of deafness was close to zero as the patients had still residual hearing. Between the groups A-C no significant difference we found in duration of deafness.

Speech comprehension

3- months interval.

1. FMT Scores: After 3 months of device use, groups B-D (ES-only) had the following mean scores: FLEX20 ES 35.11% (±24.29%), FLEX24 ES: 38.13% (±24.22%), and FLEX28 ES: 52.57% (±22.34%). The subjects using EAS scored a mean 55.17% (±18.72%). The median for EAS users was: 55.0%. For the ES-only groups the median was: FLEX20 ES: 30.0%, FLEX24 ES: 30.0%, and FLEX28 ES: 55.0% (Fig 2A). EAS subjects scored significantly better than FLEX20 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 87.5, n1 = 15 n2 = 23, p = 0.011) and FLEX24 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 103, n1 = 15 n2 = 24, p = 0.026). FLEX28 ES users scored significantly better than FLEX20 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 235, n1 = 23 n2 = 35, p = 0.008) and FLEX24 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 280.5, n1 = 24 n2 = 35, p = 0.031). The scores of the EAS subjects and FLEX28 ES users did not show significant differences.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Median, interquartile, and minimum and maximum scores after 3 months, statistical significance is marked with * for p<0.05 and ** for p<0.01 A) Freiburg Monosyllables, B) HSM sentences in quiet, C) HSM sentences in noise.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174900.g002

2. HSM in quiet: After 3 months of device use, groups B-D (ES-only) had the following mean scores: FLEX20 ES: 51.98% (±37.22%), FLEX24 ES: 54.58% (±31.47%), and FLEX28 ES: 74.86% (±26.36%). The subjects using EAS scored a mean 83.87% (±16.96%). The median for EAS users was: 88.6%. For the ES-only groups the median was: FLEX20 ES: 62.2%, FLEX24 ES: 53.77%, and FLEX28 ES: 80.66% (Fig 2B). EAS users scored significantly better than FLEX20 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 79, n1 = 15 n2 = 23, p = 0.05) and FLEX24 ES users (Mann–Whitney: U = 80, n1 = 15 n2 = 24, p = 0.004). FLEX28 ES users scored significantly better than FLEX20 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 255, n1 = 23 n2 = 34, p = 0.027) and FLEX24 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 256.5, n1 = 24 n2 = 34, p = 0.017). The scores of the EAS subjects and FLEX28 ES users did not show significant differences.

3. HSM in noise: After 3 months of device use, groups B-D (ES-only) had the following mean scores: FLEX20 ES: 21.13% (±25.00%), FLEX24 ES: 28.97% (±29.76%), and FLEX28 ES: 41.51% (±24.71%). The subjects using EAS scored a mean 50.50% (±26.52%). The median for EAS users was: 46.7%. For the ES-only groups the median was: FLEX20 ES: 15.09%, FLEX24 ES: 20.75%, and FLEX28 ES: 42.4% (Fig 2C). EAS users scored significantly better than FLEX20 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 60.5, n1 = 15 n2 = 20, p = 0.003) and FLEX24 ES users (Mann–Whitney: U = 81, n1 = 15 n2 = 19, p = 0.033). FLEX28 ES users scored significantly better than FLEX20 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 162, n1 = 20 n2 = 31, p = 0.004). The scores of the EAS subjects and FLEX28 ES users did not show significant differences.

6- months interval.

1. FMT Scores: After 6 months of device use, groups B-D (ES-only) had the following mean scores: FLEX20 ES: 42.63% (±23.78%), FLEX24 ES: 47.29% (±23.77%), and FLEX28 ES: 49.09% (±20.06%). The subjects using EAS scored a mean 56.92% (±19.74%). No significant differences were found between groups. The median for EAS users was: 55.0%. For the ES-only groups the median was: FLEX20 ES: 40.0%, FLEX24 ES: 52.5%, and FLEX28 ES: 50.0% (Fig 3A). The scores of the EAS subjects and FLEX28 ES users did not show significant differences.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Median, interquartile, and minimum and maximum scores after 6 months: A) Freiburg Monosyllables, B) HSM sentences in quiet, C) HSM sentences in noise, statistical significance is marked with * for p<0.05 and ** for p<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174900.g003

2. HSM in quiet: After 6 months of device use, groups B-D (ES-only) had the following mean scores: FLEX20 ES: 57.86% (±34.00%), FLEX24 ES: 68.80% (±27.17%), and FLEX28 ES: 74.35% (±28.08%). The subjects using EAS scored a mean 88.80% (±17.55%). The median for EAS users was: 97.13%. For the ES-only groups the median was: FLEX20 ES: 64.15%, FLEX24 ES: 79.24%, and FLEX28 ES: 88.67% (Fig 3B). EAS users scored significantly better than FLEX20 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 41, n1 = 12 n2 = 19, p = 0.003), FLEX24 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 60.5, n1 = 12, n2 = 23, p = 0.007), and FLEX28 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 115.5, n1 = 12, n2 = 33, p = 0.034).

3. HSM in noise: After 6 months of device use, groups B-D (ES-only) had the following mean scores: FLEX20 ES: 32.96% (±32.78%), FLEX24 ES: 38.65% (±25.40%), and FLEX28 ES: 49.44% (±26.18%). The subjects using EAS scored a mean 59.75% (±19.9%). The median for EAS users was: 58.88%. For the ES-only groups the median was: FLEX20 ES: 25.47%, FLEX24 ES: 45.28%, and FLEX28 ES: 52.83% (Fig 3C). EAS users scored significantly better than FLEX20 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 56, n1 = 12, n2 = 17, p = 0.041) and FLEX24 ES users (Mann–Whitney U = 64, n1 = 12, n2 = 19, p = 0.042). The scores of the EAS subjects and FLEX28 ES users did not show significant differences.

Comparison of the 3- and 6- months results.

Intragroup comparison of the 3- and 6- months FMT results showed a significant difference only in the FLEX24 ES group, which had a significantly higher score at 6 months than at 3 months (Wilcoxon signed-rank: T = 50.5, p = 0.023) (Fig 4A).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Median, interquartile, and minimum and maximum scores between scores after 3 and 6 months: A) Freiburg Monosyllables, B) HSM sentences in quiet, C) HSM sentences in noise.

Significant differences of 3- and 6 months data and intragroup comparison are marked with * for p<0.05 and ** for p<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174900.g004

Intragroup comparison of the 3- and 6- months HSM-sentence test in quiet scores showed a significant difference only in the FLEX24 ES group, which had a significantly higher score at 6 months than at 3 months (Wilcoxon signed-rank: T = 32.5, p = 0.012) (Fig 4B). FLEX20 ES and FLEX24 ES groups had a higher distribution of results than did the EAS and FLEX28 ES groups.

Intragroup comparison of the 3- and 6- months HSM-sentence test in noise scores showed a significant difference only in the FLEX20 ES group, which had a significantly higher score at 6 months than at 3 months (Wilcoxon signed-rank: T = 32.5, p = 0.041) (Fig 4C).

Influence of demographic factors on the speech data

A hierarchical multiple regression analyses was performed in order to investigate the effect of demographic factors on the speech data and to determine if the group factor remains significant when including these confounding factors. Age of implantation and duration of hearing impairment were investigated as probable confounding factors. Exemplarily, the regression analysis was performed for the speech data of the FMT at 3 months. In the model the FMT was the dependent variable.

In the first step the regression analysis was performed with the group variables (A-D) as independent variables. Three dummy variables D1-D3 were defined for each ES group (group B- group D). The EAS group (group A) was defined as the baseline group and is specified when the three dummy variables are set to zero. Therefore, the effect of the dummy variables shows the effect of the ES groups in comparison to the EAS group.

Including the dummy variables has a significant effect on the model and explains 12.4% of the variance (R2 = 0.124, F (3, 92) = 4.32, p = 0.007). The model constant b0, is 55.17 and corresponds to the mean value of the FMT score of group A. The model coefficients b, which are the slopes of the linear model, and the standardized coefficients β for the dummy variables are: D1: group B vs. group A: b = -20.05, β = -0.35 (t = -2.61, p = 0.010); D2: group C vs. group A: b = -17.04, β = -0.31 (t = -2.26, p = 0.026); D3: group D vs. group A: b = -2.60, β = -0.052 (t = -0.37, p = 0.715)

This shows that entering of D1 and D2 has a negative significant effect on the speech score. It means that the speech scores for group B and group C are significantly worse than those of group A. For Group D, no significant change compared to group A was found. This is in line with the findings of comparing the group medians.

When adding age of implantation as a predictor, the model lead to a significant increase in the prediction of variability (Fchange (1, 91) = 8.23, p = 0.005), and the overall model explains 19.6% of the variance (R2 = 0.196, F (4, 91) = 5.56, p <0.001). The model constant b0 changes to 81.44. The model coefficients b and the standardized coefficients β for age of implantation are b = -0.46, β = -2.73 (t = -2.87, p = 0.005), and change for the dummy variables to: D1: group B vs. group A: b = -17.45, β = -0.31 (t = -2.34, p = 0.021); D2: group C vs. group A: b = -14.28, β = -0.26 (t = -1.95, p = 0.055); D3: group D vs. group A: b = 0.53, β = 0.11 (t = 0.77, p = 0.939).

This shows that age of implantation has a negative significant effect on the speech scores, whereas the effect of dummy variable D1 stays significant and D2 still shows a clear trend if including age of implantation into the model. This means that the effect of the groups is slightly decreased when adding age of implantation, but the group variables still explain most of the model variance.

Adding duration of hearing impairment as a predictor did not show an additional significant effect on the model (Fchange (1, 91) = 8.23, p = 0.005). It explains 19.9% of the variance (R2 = 0.199, F (4, 91) = 4.49, p = 0.001). The model constant b0 changes to 83.55. The model coefficients b and the standardized coefficients β are b = -0.90, β = -0.54 (t = -0.57, p = 0.572) for duration of hearing impairment and b = -0.46, β = -2.74 (t = -2.87, p = 0.005) for age of implantation, and they change for the dummy variables to D1: group B vs. group A: b = -17.15, β = -0.30 (t = -2.29, p = 0.024); D2: group C vs. group A: b = -14.24, β = -0.26 (t = -1.94, p = 0.056); D3: group D vs. group A: b = 0.41, β = 0.08 (t = 0.59, p = 0.953).

This means that duration of hearing impairment does not affect the speech scores and does not change the effect of the group variables and of age of implantation on the speech scores.

Discussion

We analyzed the speech understanding outcomes of subjects who had undergone residual hearing preservation CI surgery using MED-EL implants with different lengths of FLEX electrodes. In 2006, Hamzavi & Arnoldner [16] showed by deactivating apical electrodes in subjects implanted with the 31.5 mm length standard MED-EL electrode that deep stimulation of the cochlea enhances speech understanding. However, as the different conditions in this particular study were tested acutely without time for adaptation, the results are of limited significance only. Buchman et al. [17], who examined speech understanding in subjects with MED-EL STANDARD (length 31.5 mm) or MEDIUM (length 24 mm) electrodes, showed that insertion with the longer electrode offered better speech perception performance, at least in the early postactivation period. However, subjective measurements like quality of life and music perception showed no difference between the cohorts. The authors suspected that deeper insertion angles with the standard arrays produce greater degrees of cochlear coverage in the apical regions and a better or more natural tonotopic place representation during stimulation. In this context, Landsberger et. al [31] evaluated data from 92 cochlear implantations and found that longer electrode arrays, in particular the MED-EL STANDARD and FLEX28, demonstrated much smaller deviations from the spiral ganglion map [32] than other electrode arrays. Our study also demonstrates better hearing results with longer electrode arrays—in particular the FLEX28—in the ES-only user group, which might in fact be attributable to the more natural frequency mapping. This hypothesis is supported by very recent data from Rader et al., who found more accurate pitch perception in cochlear implant subjects when the place of stimulation and the stimulation rate had been matched [33]. Also, there seems to be growing evidence that the apical region of the cochlear is more suitable for conveying adequate rate pitch for frequencies up to 300 Hz, thus giving patients with longer arrays and appropriate coding strategies access to low frequency information which is unavailable to short electrode subjects. Some studies [34, 35] suggested that temporal processing near the apex may be different compared to more basal locations in the cochlear. However, insertion depth has to be considered carefully on a case by case basis and overly deep insertions of short electrode arrays should be avoided, as there is evidence that the more basal parts of the cochlear still seem to be more relevant for speech understanding than the apical regions [36, 25, 26].

Another plausible explanation for better hearing in the subjects with longer arrays simply might be the larger electrode contact spacing with those electrodes leading to less crosstalk or channel interaction between adjacent contacts. There is evidence that less channel interaction leads to improved spectral resolution and better speech understanding in noise [37, 38, 39].

Our conclusions on this topic are obviously based on the data from the three groups with no usable acoustic hearing, namely FLEX28 ES, FLEX24 ES and FLEX20 ES. Although only 3- and 6- months data are available so far, it is apparent that greater insertion angles do significantly enhance speech understanding in these ES-only users, especially at the 3- months interval. Even at the 6- months appointment, subjects with the FLEX28 ES tendentially outperformed the ES-only FLEX20 ES and FLEX24 ES users, most notably in the two HSM tests, although the score differences did not reach significance. Also, the performance variation among the subjects was much smaller in the FLEX28 ES group than in the other two ES-only groups (Figs 3 and 4). From our clinical routine data, there is empirical evidence that ES-only subjects with conventional electrodes outperform ES-only subjects with a short electrode array (i.e. 20 mm and shorter). Such pronounced differences as seen in our current analysis were, however, not expected, partly because of the suggestion of a possible accommodation to shorter electrode lengths leading to comparable hearing performance after a few months [6]. Although the scores of the FLEX20 ES and FLEX24 ES groups were catching up to that of the FLEX28 ES group at 6 months after surgery, a noticeable difference nonetheless remained. It could be speculated that, although brain plasticity can compensate for the pitch-mismatch with short electrode arrays over time to a great extent, there are still limits to what it can actually achieve. In this context it is worth mentioning recent work by Reiss et al. [40], who demonstrated in subjects with residual acoustic hearing in the contralateral ear that the pitch fusion of the electric stimulus and an acoustic tone gets broader with increasing pitch-mismatch between the acoustic and electric modality, suggesting that brain plasticity—although apparently existing—has its limitations. Further observation of the study subjects would be very interesting to see if the FLEX20 ES and FLEX24 ES groups would eventually fully catch up with the FLEX28 ES group. In this context, it would also be interesting to include measures for listening-effort into the test battery, something which is currently being discussed at our center.

Subjects implanted with the shorter arrays had higher degrees of acoustic hearing prior to surgery, which was why they had been implanted with shorter electrode types (Fig 1A). Still, 3 months after initial switch-on, the HSM in noise test scores of subjects with the FLEX20 ES electrode who had lost usable residual hearing (i.e. the ES-only subjects with a FLEX20), were barely half of those of the FLEX28 ES group. It is also worth mentioning that the FLEX28 ES group (without acoustic hearing) and the EAS users group (with functional residual hearing) achieved very similar results on the HSM sentences in noise test. This is remarkable because EAS stimulation is generally considered far superior to electric stimulation only [41, 42]. However, when looking at subjects with comparable demographics, in particular preoperative residual hearing, the picture is not as clear: the advantage for the EAS subjects over the FLEX28 ES subjects is only marginal. One might even be tempted to play the devil’s advocate and argue that possible shortcomings of the shallow insertion of EAS electrodes are only just outweighed by the users being able to use their acoustic residual hearing. In other words: EAS stimulation might not give a huge benefit over conventional electric stimulation with longer electrodes, at least as reflected in speech test scores from tests conducted in artificial settings, i.e. in clinical testing. Of course, there is no substitute for natural acoustic hearing and EAS subjects still have advantages when it comes to listening to music [43, 44]. EAS subjects also seem to need to expend less effort when listening in multi-talker environments, where it is key to perceive high-fidelity low-frequency information in order to separate individual talkers from each other and to listen to the intended target speaker [41, 42, 43, 45, 46]. Furthermore, bimodal hearing subjects also usually show measurable improvements over hearing with a CI alone [47, 48, 49, 50]. On the other hand, in the bimodal group, the effect of listening with two input channels, i.e. two ears, one supplied with a hearing aid and one with a conventional CI, also needs to be taken into account as a possible reason for improved hearing scores in the bimodal condition, rather than simply the combination of electric and acoustic hearing alone. However, the current dataset shows the dilemma that subjects and clinicians are currently in: on one hand, residual hearing should be preserved to the highest possible extent and today this is only possible with comparatively short electrode arrays; on the other hand, when residual hearing is lost, either through surgery or over time, CI users with short arrays seem to be limited in their hearing capabilities compared to CI users with longer electrodes. An investigation into EAS hearing in subjects implanted with longer electrode arrays and good postoperative residual hearing would be highly useful in shedding light on this topic. This combination is rare, but occasionally we do have subjects who explicitly opt for the long electrode array despite having relatively good residual hearing. This group, however is too small at present to draw any conclusions. Also, it needs to be investigated if deeper insertion of longer electrodes, and with it the stimulation of more apical structures of the cochlea, is indeed the reason for the superior performance, or if the larger electrode spacing together with better channel separation is the key for the apparently better speech understanding of subjects implanted with longer electrode arrays.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Cornelia Batsoulis, Ph.D, MED-EL Medical Electronics Hannover, for her scientific support in this retrospective study.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: AI AB TL.
  2. Data curation: AI AB.
  3. Formal analysis: AI AB.
  4. Investigation: AI AB.
  5. Methodology: AI AB TL.
  6. Project administration: AI.
  7. Supervision: TL AB.
  8. Validation: OM.
  9. Visualization: AI AB.
  10. Writing – original draft: AI AB OM.
  11. Writing – review & editing: AI AB.

References

  1. 1. von Ilberg CA, Baumann U, Kiefer J, Tillein J, Adunka OF. Electric-acoustic stimulation of the auditory system: a review of the first decade. Audiol Neurootol. 2011;16 Suppl 2:1–30. Epub 2011 May 24.
  2. 2. Helbig S; Baumann U; Helbig M; von Malsen-Waldkirch N; Gstoettner W. A new combined speech processor for electric and acoustic stimulation—eight months experience. ORL J Otorhinolaryngol Relat Spec. 2008;70(6):359–65. Epub 2008 Nov 4. pmid:18984971
  3. 3. Helms Tillery K, Brown CA, Bacon SP. Comparing the effects of reverberation and noise on speech recognition in simulated electric-acoustic listening. J Acoust Soc Am 2012;131:416–23. pmid:22280603
  4. 4. Büchner A, Schüssler M, Battmer RD, Stöver T, Lesinski-Schiedat A, Lenarz T. Impact of low-frequency hearing. Audiol Neurotol 2009;14 Suppl 1:8–13.
  5. 5. Lenarz T, Stöver T, Buechner A, Paasche G, Briggs R, Risi F, et al. Temporal bone results and hearing preservation with a new straight electrode. Audiol Neurotol 2006;11 Suppl 1:34–41.
  6. 6. Gantz BJ, Turner CW. Combining acoustic and electrical hearing. Laryngoscope 2003;113:1726–30. pmid:14520097
  7. 7. Pelliccia P, Venail F, Bonafé A, Makeieff M, Iannetti G, Bartolomeo M, et al. Cochlea size variability and implications in clinical practice. Acta Otorhinolaryngol Ital 2014;34:42–9. pmid:24711682
  8. 8. Franke-Trieger A, Jolly C, Darbinjan A, Zahnert T, Mürbe D. Insertion depth angles of cochlear implant arrays with varying length: a temporal bone study. Otol Neurotol 2014;35:58–63. pmid:24335932
  9. 9. van der Marel KS, Briaire JJ, Wolterbeek R, Snel-Bongers J, Verbist BM, Frijns JH. Diversity in cochlear morphology and its influence on cochlear implant electrode position. Ear Hear 2014;35:e9–20. pmid:24196418
  10. 10. Trieger A, Schulze A, Schneider M, Zahnert T, Mürbe D. In vivo measurements of the insertion depth of cochlear implant arrays using flat-panel volume computed tomography. Otol Neurotol 2011;32:152–7. pmid:20962701
  11. 11. Causon A, Verschuur C, Newman TA. A Retrospective Analysis of the Contribution of Reported Factors in Cochlear Implantation on Hearing Preservation Outcomes. Otol Neurotol. 2015 Aug;36(7):1137–45. pmid:25853614
  12. 12. Skarżyński H, Matusiak M, Furmanek M, Skarżyński PH. Results of SRA Nucleus Freedom CI in population of children with functional residual hearing. Cochlear Implants Int 2014;15 Suppl 1:S24–6.
  13. 13. James CJ, Fraysse B, Deguine O, Lenarz T, Mawman D, Ramos A, et al. Combined electroacoustic stimulation in conventional candidates for cochlear implantation. Audiol Neurootol 2006; 11 Suppl 1:57–62.
  14. 14. Helbig S, Baumann U, Hey C, Helbig M. Hearing preservation after complete cochlear coverage in cochlear implantation with the free-fitting FLEXSOFT electrode carrier. Otol Neurotol 2011;32:973–9. pmid:21730882
  15. 15. Tamir S, Ferrary E, Borel S, Sterkers O, Bozorg Grayeli A. Hearing preservation after cochlear implantation using deeply inserted FLEX atraumatic electrode arrays. Audiol Neurootol 2012;17:331–7. pmid:22813984
  16. 16. Hamzavi J, Arnoldner C. Effect of deep insertion of the cochlear implant electrode array on pitch estimation and speech perception. Acta Otolaryngol 2006;126:1182–7. pmid:17050311
  17. 17. Buchman CA, Dillon MT, King ER, Adunka MC, Adunka OF, Pillsbury HC. Influence of cochlear implant insertion depth on performance: a prospective randomized trial. Otol Neurotol 2014;35:1773–9. pmid:25122601
  18. 18. Yukawa K, Cohen L, Blamey P, Pyman B, Tungvachirakul V, O’Leary S. Effects of insertion depth of cochlear implant electrodes upon speech perception. Audiol Neurootol 2004;9:163–72. pmid:15084821
  19. 19. Skinner MW, Ketten DR, Holden LK, Harding GW, Smith PG, Gates GA, et al. CT-derived estimation of cochlear morphology and electrode array position in relation to word recognition in Nucleus-22 recipients. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 2002;3:332–50. pmid:12382107
  20. 20. Fu QJ, Shannon RV. Effects of electrode configuration and frequency allocation on vowel recognition with the Nucleus-22 cochlear implant. Ear Hear 1999;20:332–44. pmid:10466569
  21. 21. Friesen LM, Shannon RV, Slattery WH 3rd. Effects of electrode location on speech recognition with the Nucleus-22 cochlear implant. J Am Acad Audiol 2000;11:418–28. pmid:11012237
  22. 22. Gani M, Valentini G, Sigrist A, Kos M, Boex C. Implications of deep electrode insertion on cochlear implant fitting. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol, 8, 2007, p. 69–83 pmid:17216585
  23. 23. Van der Marel KS, Briaire JJ, Verbist BM, Muurling TJ, Frijns JH: The Influence of Cochlear Implant Electrode position on Performance. Audiol Neurootol. 2015;20(3):202–11. Epub 2015 Apr 28. pmid:25924706
  24. 24. Lazard DS, Vincent C, Venail F, Van de Heyning P, Truy E, Sterkers O, et al. Pre-Per and Postoperative Factors Affecting Performance of Postlinguistically Deaf Adults Using Cochlear Implants: A New Conceptual Model Over Time PLoS One. 2012;7(11):e48739. Epub 2012 Nov 9. pmid:23152797
  25. 25. Kos MI, Boex C, Guyot JP, Pelizzone M. Partial withdrawal of deeply inserted cochlear electrodes: observations of two patients. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2007 Nov;264(11):1369–72. Epub 2007 Jun 12. pmid:17562059
  26. 26. Finley CC, Holden TA, Holden LK, Whiting BR, Chole RA, Neely GJ, Hullar TE, Skinner MW. Role of electrode placement as a contributor to variability in cochlear implant outcomes. Otol Neurotol. 2008 Oct;29(7):920–8. pmid:18667935
  27. 27. Wilson B, Finley C, Lawson D, Wolford R, Eddington D, Rabinowitz W. Better speech recognition with cochlear implants Nature, 352, 1991, p. 236–238 pmid:1857418
  28. 28. Hochmair I, Nopp P, Jolly C, Schmidt M, Schosser H, Garnham C, Anderson I. Trends Amplif, MED-EL Cochlear implants: state of the art and a glimpse into the future 10(4), 2006 Dec, p. 201–219
  29. 29. Lehnhardt E. Sprachaudiometrie. In: Lehnhardt E, Laszig R, eds. Praxis der Audiometrie [in German]. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme-Verlag, 2001; 173–196.
  30. 30. Hochmair-Desoyer I, Schulz E, Moser L, Schmidt M. The HSM sentence test as a tool for evaluating the speech understanding in noise of cochlear implant users. Am J Otol 1997;18: S83. pmid:9391610
  31. 31. Landsberger DM, Svrakic M, Roland JT Jr, and Svirsky MA. The Relationship between Insertion Angles, Default Frequency Allocations, and Spiral Ganglion Place Pitch in Cochlear Implants, Ear Hear. 2015 Sep-Oct;36(5):e207–13 pmid:25860624
  32. 32. Stakhovskaya O., Sridhar D., Bonham BH, and Leake PA. Frequency map for the human cochlear spiral ganglion: implications for cochlear implants, J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 2007. 8, 220–233. pmid:17318276
  33. 33. Rader T, Döge J, Adel Y, Weissgerber T, Baumann U. Place dependent stimulation rates improve pitch perception in cochlear implantees with single-sided deafness. Hear Res. 2016 Jul 1;339:94–103. [Epub ahead of print] pmid:27374479
  34. 34. Schatzer R, Vermeire K, Visser D, Krenmayr A, Kals M, Voormolen M, Van de Heyning P, and Zierhofer C. Electric-acoustic pitch comparisons in single-sided-deaf cochlear implant users: Frequency-place functions and rate pitch, Hear Res 2014. 309C, 26–35.
  35. 35. Landsberger DM, Vermeire K, Claes A, Van Rompaey V, Van de Heyning P. Qualities of Single Electrode Stimulation as a Function of Rate and Place of Stimulation with a Cochlear Implant. Ear Hear. 2016 May-Jun;37(3):e149–59. pmid:26583480
  36. 36. Lenarz T, Büchner A, Tasche C, Cristofoli T, Lesinski-Schiedat A, Wallenberg EV, Battmer RD, Busby PA, Frohne C. The results in patients implanted with the nucleus double array cochlear implant: pitch discrimination and auditory performance. Ear Hear. 2002 Feb;23(1 Suppl):90S–101S. pmid:11883772
  37. 37. Berenstein CK, Mens LH, Mulder JJ, and Vanpoucke FJ. Current steering and current focusing in cochlear implants: comparison of monopolar, tripolar, and virtual channel electrode configurations, Ear Hear 2008. 29, 250–260. pmid:18595189
  38. 38. Smith ZM, Parkinson WS, and Long CJ. Multipolar current focusing increases spectral resolution in cochlear implants, IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. 2013
  39. 39. Srinivasan AG, Padilla M, Shannon RV, and Landsberger DM. Improving speech perception in noise with current focusing in cochlear implant users, Hear Res (2013). 299C, 29–36.
  40. 40. Reiss LA, Ito RA, Eggleston JL, Wozny DR. Abnormal binaural spectral integration in cochlear implant users. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2014 Apr;15(2):235–48. Epub 2014 Jan 24. pmid:24464088
  41. 41. Helbig S, Baumann U, Helbig M, von Malsen-Waldkirch N, Gstoettner W. A new combined speech processor for electric and acoustic stimulation-eight months experience. ORL J Otorhinolaryngol Relat Spec 2008;70:359–65. pmid:18984971
  42. 42. Dorman MF, Gifford RH. Combining acoustic and electric stimulation in the service of speech recognition. Int J Audiol 2010;49:912–9. pmid:20874053
  43. 43. Gfeller KE, Olszewski C, Turner C, Gantz B, Oleson J. Music perception with cochlear implants and residual hearing. Audiol Neurootol 2006;11 Suppl 1:12–5.
  44. 44. Brockmeier SJ; Peterreins M; Lorens A; Vermeire K; Helbig S; Anderson I; et al. Music Perception in Electric Acoustic Stimulation Users as Assessed by the Mu.S.I.C. Test Adv Otorhinolaryngol, 67, 2010, p. 70–80 pmid:19955723
  45. 45. Turner CW, Gantz BJ, Vidal C, Behrens A, Henry BA. Speech recognition in noise for cochlear implant listeners: benefits of residual acoustic hearing. J Acoust Soc Am 2004;115: 1729–35. pmid:15101651
  46. 46. Lorens A, Polak M, Piotrowska A, Skarzynski H. Outcomes of treatment of partial deafness with cochlear implantation: a DUET study. Laryngoscope 2008;118:288–94. pmid:18000465
  47. 47. Armstrong M, Pegg P, James C, Blamey P. Speech perception in noise with implant and hearing aid. Am J Otol 1997;18(6 Suppl):S140–1. pmid:9391635
  48. 48. Tyler RS, Parkinson AJ, Wilson BS, Witt S, Preece JP, Noble W. Patients utilizing a hearing aid and a cochlear implant: speech perception and localization. Ear Hear 2002; 23:98–105. pmid:11951854
  49. 49. Ching TY, Incerti P, Hill M. Binaural benefits for adults who use hearing aids and cochlear implants in opposite ears. Ear Hear 2004;25:9–21. pmid:14770014
  50. 50. Illg A, Bojanowicz M, Lesinski-Schiedat A, Lenarz T, Büchner A. Evaluation of the bimodal benefit in a large cohort of cochlear implant subjects using a contralateral hearing aid. Otol Neurotol 2014;35:e240–4. pmid:25058838