Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-27gpq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T21:47:55.541Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The neurobiology of aggression and violence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2015

Daniel R. Rosell
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Icahn Medical School, Mount Sinai, New York, New York, USA Special Evaluation Program of Mood and Personality Disorders, Icahn Medical School, Mount Sinai, New York, New York, USA
Larry J. Siever*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Icahn Medical School, Mount Sinai, New York, New York, USA Special Evaluation Program of Mood and Personality Disorders, Icahn Medical School, Mount Sinai, New York, New York, USA Department of Psychiatry and the VISN3 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center (MIRECC), James J. Peters VA Medical Center, Bronx, New York, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: Larry J. Siever, MD, James J. Peters Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, 130 West Kingsbridge Road, Room 6A–44, Bronx, NY 10468, USA. (Email: larry.siever@va.gov).
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aggression and violence represent a significant public health concern and a clinical challenge for the mental healthcare provider. A great deal has been revealed regarding the neurobiology of violence and aggression, and an integration of this body of knowledge will ultimately serve to advance clinical diagnostics and therapeutic interventions. We will review here the latest findings regarding the neurobiology of aggression and violence. First, we will introduce the construct of aggression, with a focus on issues related to its heterogeneity, as well as the importance of refining the aggression phenotype in order to reduce pathophysiologic variability. Next we will examine the neuroanatomy of aggression and violence, focusing on regional volumes, functional studies, and interregional connectivity. Significant emphasis will be on the amygdala, as well as amygdala–frontal circuitry. Then we will turn our attention to the neurochemistry and molecular genetics of aggression and violence, examining the extensive findings on the serotonergic system, as well as the growing literature on the dopaminergic and vasopressinergic systems. We will also address the contribution of steroid hormones, namely, cortisol and testosterone. Finally, we will summarize these findings with a focus on reconciling inconsistencies and potential clinical implications; and, then we will suggest areas of focus for future directions in the field.

Type
Review Articles
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2015 

Introduction

Violence and aggression are pervasive phenomena, and represent a significant public health burden.Reference Hall, Simon, Lee and Mercy 1 Reference Whitaker 4 Aggression and violence are also common symptomatic manifestations that the mental health professional is frequently called upon to identify, evaluate, and treat. While aggression and violence have multiple determinants—including social and psychological factors—characterizing neurobiological correlates of aggression may ultimately lead to the development of clinically informative biomarkers and rational treatment design.

In this review, we will discuss advances in the neurobiology of aggression and violence, addressing structural and functional neuroanatomical findings, as well as those related to neurochemistry and molecular genetics. Studies from the human literature will be our primary focus; however, preclinical or animal studies will also be discussed when there is limited human data and/or they are of a high-impact nature.

We will first examine the construct of aggression, with a specific focus on dimensional and categorical definitions, and the importance of refining the aggression phenotype under study in order to reduce neurobiological variability. The role of phenomenological heterogeneity as a potential source of inconsistencies between studies will be highlighted throughout this review. We will then review neuroanatomical findings, including volumetric, functional, and connectivity studies, with a particular emphasis on the role of the amygdala, limbic prefrontal regions, and amygdala-prefrontal circuitry. Following will be a discussion of the neurochemistry, neuropharmacology, and molecular genetics of aggression, with a primary focus on the serotonin system, as well as an examination of the less well developed literature related to dopamine, vasopressin, and the steroid hormones, cortisol and testosterone. Last, we will attempt to integrate these findings and reconcile inconsistencies in the field, and make recommendations for future studies, including potential clinical applications.

Aggression Subtypes and Dimensional Considerations

Impulsive and instrumental aggression

Aggression is a complex and heterogeneous construct, which in all likelihood serves as a significant source of variability between neurobiological studies. Therefore, it is essential to identify subtypes or classes of aggression, as well as subordinate factors and associated traits (eg, impulsivity, callousness). A longstanding system for codifying aggression has been the impulsive/instrumental dichotomy: the former being an abrupt, “hot-headed” response to a perceived threat or provocation; and the latter being premeditated, goal-oriented, and “cold-blooded.” In the clinical realm, aggression with the qualities of negative affect and impulsivity is the form that characteristically presents, whereas aggression of a more premeditated and instrumental nature is typically addressed in the forensic setting. The primary form of aggression that will be discussed in this review, and the one that has received the greatest attention in neurobiological studies, is the impulsive variant.

While highly representative manifestations of either impulsive or instrumental aggression certainly do occur, it is not uncommon that assaultive and violent acts can exhibit both impulsive and instrumental features.Reference Bushman and Anderson 5 For example, aggression can occur in a sudden and unplanned manner, in response to a perceived provocation, with a hostile or angry affect, but, nevertheless, in a highly controlled and purposeful manner, with an explicit and tangible objective, eg, intimidation, or elevation of one’s self-esteem. Thus, as we will discuss throughout this review, further refinement of the quality of aggression being studied—beyond the impulsive/instrumental distinction—we believe is critical to isolating underlying neurobiological mechanisms. Along these lines, we will next discuss the “reactive and proactive” classification of aggression.

Reactive and proactive aggression

Although similar to the impulsive/instrumental distinction, the reactive/proactive classification of aggression has been more formally operationalized and extensively validated, initially in child and adolescent populations,Reference Baker, Raine, Liu and Jacobson 6 Reference Raine, Dodge and Loeber 11 but more recently in adults.Reference Cima, Raine, Meesters and Popma 8 , Reference Gardner, Archer and Jackson 12 , Reference Lobbestael, Cima and Arntz 13 This system does not view these 2 forms of aggression categorically or diametrically related, but rather, assumes that they coexist and contribute together to an individual’s “total” level of aggression, and each is assessed dimensionally.

The reactive subtype best resembles what the impulsive category was originally intended to capture, ie, aggression that (a) is invariably accompanied by anger, rage, or hostility; (b) occurs in response to frustration or perceived provocation (particularly in an interpersonal context); and (c) is motivated by the more rudimentary purpose of quelling unpleasant affect states. Proactive aggression, on the other hand, characteristically (a) does not invariably involve a negative affect state such as anger or rage; (b) is typically initiated by the offender, rather than provoked; and (c) is explicitly motivated by an expectation of obtaining something of value, eg, an object, reward, power, status, or social dominance.

Reactive and proactive forms of aggression frequently coexist and are highly intercorrelated. Moreover, these 2 forms of aggression have been observed in adult populations with borderlineReference Gardner, Archer and Jackson 12 and antisocialReference Lobbestael, Cima and Arntz 13 personality disorders (BPD and ASPD). Both reactive and proactive aggression subtypes have also been found to contribute to a common self-report measure of trait aggression in adults, ie, the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ), which is frequently used in studies of impulsive aggression.Reference Cima, Raine, Meesters and Popma 8 Thus, both forms likely are present to varying degrees in clinical populations as well as in research cohorts with severe/pathological aggression.

Despite the concurrence of reactive and proactive aggression, these 2 subtypes also exhibit important divergent properties: Reactive aggression is associated with a history of abuse,Reference Dodge, Lochman, Harnish, Bates and Pettit 14 , Reference Kolla, Malcolm, Attard, Arenovich, Blackwood and Hodgins 15 negative emotionality, and impulsivity,Reference Cima, Raine, Meesters and Popma 8 , Reference Raine, Dodge and Loeber 11 and it is also somewhat negatively predicted by callous-unemotional (CU) traits (a component of psychopathy).Reference Cima, Raine, Meesters and Popma 8 Proactive aggression, on the other hand, is associated positively with psychopathy,Reference Kolla, Malcolm, Attard, Arenovich, Blackwood and Hodgins 15 physical aggression, and violent offenses.Reference Cima, Raine, Meesters and Popma 8 Furthermore, the tendency to over-attribute hostile intent to social cues, or hostile attribution bias, is related to reactive but not proactive aggression.Reference Arsenio, Adams and Gold 16 Reference Hubbard, Dodge, Cillessen, Coie and Schwartz 19 On the other hand, the belief that violence and assaultive behavior will lead to a favorable outcome, or positive outcome expectancy, is associated specifically with proactive aggression.Reference Smithmyer, Hubbard and Simons 20 , Reference Walters 21 Finally, attentional interference by aggression-associated stimuli is directly and inversely correlated with reactive and proactive aggression, respectively.Reference Brugman, Lobbestael and Arntz 22 Therefore, it is very likely that both forms are present, to varying degrees, in commonly studied, pathological manifestations of aggression, and may be a significant source of inconsistency between studies.

Aggression: Categorical Diagnoses and Intermittent Explosive Disorder

Categorical diagnoses

The role of categorical diagnoses is also a critical issue in studying the neurobiology of violence and assaultive behavior. Aggression and violence can occur as a function of a number of major psychiatric syndromal disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD); attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); bipolar disorder, schizophrenia (SCZ), and related psychotic illnesses; alcohol/substance abuse; and neuropsychiatric conditions (eg, dementia, autoimmune encephalitis). Examining aggression in the context of these conditions, however, can make it difficult to differentiate the neurobiology of these disorders from that of aggression.

Intermittent explosive disorder

Clinically significant aggression also frequently manifests in a chronic and pervasive manner, independent of the above-described psychiatric syndromes, albeit, typically in the context of severe personality pathology. The syndrome of intermittent explosive disorder (IED) has been characterized in order to account for when this more chronic manifestation of aggression occurs with a frequency and intensity such that it contributes to significant subjective distress and/or functional impairment.Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi, Berman and Lish 23 The diagnostic criteria of IED have recently undergone a number of changes that better identify a distinctly aggressive and impaired population.Reference McCloskey, Berman, Noblett and Coccaro 24 , Reference Coccaro 25

Notable features of the latest iteration of IED criteria are (1) the requirement that the aggression be impulsive or reactive in nature; (2) inclusion of not only acts of severe physical aggression against other people or valuable property (including one’s own property, eg, cell phone), which typically occur at relatively low frequency, but also high-frequency occurrences of verbal assaultiveness and/or minor physical events that do not lead to significant harm/damage (door slamming, pushing items off a table without property damage); and (3) removal of the exclusion for BPD and ASPD, as it has been demonstrated that a diagnosis of IED is associated with a distinct biological profileReference Coccaro, Lee and Kavoussi 26 and more severe aggression and functional impairment than these personality disorder diagnoses alone.Reference McCloskey, Berman, Noblett and Coccaro 24 Last, there are also temporal criteria that allow for a state determination, ie, whether IED is current or past. The development of a more valid categorical classification of chronic, clinically severe aggression has been an important advancement for neurobiological studies.

Neuroanatomy, Functional Imaging, and Neural Circuitry

As the more impulsive and reactive forms of aggression have been the focus of neurobiological studies (as opposed to purely instrumental or premeditated manifestations), not surprisingly, regions involved in affect processing, impulse control, and emotional decision making have received the greatest attention. We will primarily focus on the extensive literature regarding the amygdala, as well as limbic prefrontal regions—namely the orbitofrontal (OFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)—in aggression. We will also introduce the growing body of structural and functional findings regarding involvement of the striatum in aggression.

Amygdala: structural and functional studies

The amygdala is a medial temporal lobe structure that plays an essential role in the integration of a wide range of sensory and motivationally salient stimuli, as well as transmission of this information to various cortical and subcortical regions. These processes underlie the amygdala’s essential role in emotionally valenced learning and memory, and the shaping of cognitive, affective, motor, and sympathetic representations and responses to affectively and motivationally salient stimuli.Reference Salzman and Fusi 27 , Reference Fernando, Murray and Milton 28

An important consideration when examining the amygdala is its basic structure and circuitry (see Figure 1 for a detailed description). Briefly, the amygdala is composed of 3 main nuclear complexes: basolateral, central or centromedial, and superficial or cortical.Reference Sah, Faber, Lopez De and Power 29 The circuitry and function of the basolateral and centromedial complexes have been the best characterized, whereas the superficial or cortical nuclei are less well understood. As depicted in Figure 1A, the basolateral complex serves as the primary locus of sensory input to the amygdala, whereas the central nucleus is considered the main output component, as it projects to numerous subcortical and brainstem structures. Intercalated cellular masses (IMs; Figure 1A) are clusters of inhibitory, GABAergic neurons nestled between the borders of the basolateral and centromedial complexes, which are believed to play a critical role inhibitory control of amygdala activity.Reference John, Bullock, Zikopoulos and Barbas 30 , Reference Lee, Kim, Kwon, Lee and Kim 31 It is worth noting that this schematic is an oversimplification, of course, and while not depicted in Figure 1A, sensory afferents also project directly to the central nucleus, particularly, visceral and gustatory, as opposed to visual, auditory, and polymodal information. Additionally, the basolateral complex also sends efferent projections to prefrontal and striatal regions, which will be addressed in a later section.

Figure 1 Amygdala: functional subdivisions and basic organization of flow of information. The amygdala is not a unitary structure, and consideration of its anatomical subdivisions and circuitry is essential. (a) One of the best characterized models of information flow and intra-amygdala circuitry involves the basolateral nuclear complex (which subsumes lateral and basal nuclei) and the central nuclear complex (which consists of a lateral [light red] and medial component [dark red]); these 2 complexes are considered the primary input and output components of this circuit, respectively. The basolateral complex receives cortical and thalamic sensory input. The lateral nucleus projects to the basal and central nuclei (lateral component); the basal nucleus projects to the central nucleus (medial component), as well as the intercalated masses (IM). The IMs are clusters of GABAergic inhibitory neurons, nestled between basolateral and central nuclei, which play a critical role in regulating central nucleus activity. The central nucleus projects to various subcortical regions, such as the hypothalamus, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), nucleus basalis (major site of acetylcholine [ACh] ascending projection neurons), and various brainstem regions that regulate neurotransmitter systems, autonomic function, innate psychomotor and visceromotor responses, and descending pain-regulation pathways. Central nucleus efferents are believed to consist of tonically active inhibitory projection neurons that activate their downstream targets when their activity is inhibited.Reference John, Bullock, Zikopoulos and Barbas 30 , Reference Lee, Kim, Kwon, Lee and Kim 31 (b)–(c) Studies of the amygdala in humans have typically considered it as a single functional unit, mainly due to its amorphous shape and relatively small size. Recent imaging studies have demonstrated the ability to differentiate the human amygdala in vivo into the 3 main nuclear complexes: basolateral, central (centromedial), and superficial (cortical).Reference Bzdok, Laird, Zilles, Fox and Eickhoff 32

Due to its relatively small size and the amorphous organization of its functional subdivisions, the amygdala has typically been assessed as a unitary structure in human imaging studies. Only until recently have the functional subdivisions of the amygdala been neuroanatomically delineated in human imaging studies (Figures 1B and 1C).Reference Bzdok, Laird, Zilles, Fox and Eickhoff 32 As will be highlighted throughout this section, taking into account the functional subdivisions of the amygdala is critical to better resolve the relationship between amygdala structure/function and aggression.

A relatively consistent finding of structural imaging findings has been an association between aggression and reduced amygdala volumes. In a study of healthy, nonclinical female participants (N=20) with levels of trait aggression not outside the normal range, Matthies et al Reference Matthies, Rusch and Weber 33 found that both right and left whole amygdala volumes were smaller in those who scored higher than the cohort median on a commonly used measure of trait aggression (Lifetime History of Aggression; LHA) relative to those with LHA scores below the cohort median. The volumes of both the right and left amygdala were also negatively correlated with LHA scores among all participants combined. Finally, specificity of this effect for aggression was supported by the absence of correlations between amygdala size and other clinical measures, viz, general psychopathology, trait anxiety, and state depression. An important caveat of these findings, however, is their applicability to clinical populations, pathological forms of aggression, and males.

Pardini et al Reference Pardini, Raine, Erickson and Loeber 34 examined amygdala volumes in men (N=56) who had been psychometrically assessed at 3 time points: (1) childhood and adolescence; (2) at the time of an MRI at 26 years of age; and (3) prospectively, approximately 3 years after the MRI. Notably, these participants manifested a broader range of aggression and psychopathology than in the Matthies et al study, described above. Similar to what Matthies and colleagues observed, however, both the left and right whole amygdala volumes were negatively correlated with a number of measures of trait aggression performed at the time of MRI in adulthood. Remarkably, aggression that had been assessed in childhood and adolescence was also negatively related to adulthood amygdala volumes. Finally, both left and right whole amygdala volumes were found to be smaller in men who engaged in violent acts within the 3 year post-scan follow up interval, even after adjusting for ratings of aggression prior to the post-scan assessment. Relations between amygdala volumes and psychopathy, a construct commonly associated with aggression, were somewhat mixed, however. Psychopathy assessments performed contemporaneously with the structural MRI in adulthood were not related to amygdala volumes; however, significant associations did exist between adulthood amygdala volumes and psychopathic features assessed in childhood/adolescence and at the 3-year post-scan follow-up assessment. Taking into account the different forms of aggression (viz, reactive and proactive—which have different relationships with psychopathy) and characterizing amygdala subregions in future studies may allow for these complex relationships to be better resolved.

In order to enhance the specificity of the relationship between amygdala structure and aggression, studies have begun to take into account the anatomical functional subdivisions of the amygdala (described earlier, and depicted in Figures 1B and 1C). Gopal et al Reference Gopal, Clark and Allgair 35 parcellated the amygdala into ventral and dorsal portions (roughly approximating the basolateral and centromedial nuclear complexes, respectively) and examined the relationship between amygdala volume and trait aggression in a mixed population of psychiatric patients with a wide range of diagnoses (N=41). Unlike the 2 studies cited above, a relationship between whole amygdala volumes and trait aggression (as assessed by the LHA) was not observed. However, planned comparisons revealed that the volume of the left dorsal amygdala, specifically, was negatively correlated with the LHA. In contrast, they found that the closely related trait, (motor) impulsivity, was positively correlated with both the right and left ventral amygdalae, but not with either dorsal subdivisions. These findings highlight the importance of taking into consideration the anatomical and hemispheric specialization of the amygdala, as well as the potential confounding role of closely related traits that may in fact have contrasting neuroanatomical correlates.

Finally, Bobes et al Reference Bobes, Ostrosky and Diaz 36 reported that in a community sample of adult men (N=54) whom were classified as either aggressive (N=25) or non-aggressive (N=29) based on a self-report measure of reactive aggression, both right and left whole amygdalae volumes were found to be smaller in the aggressive compared to the non-aggressive group. They also found that in the aggressive compared to the non-aggressive group, volumes of both the dorsal and ventral portions of the right amygdala were decreased, however, only the dorsal component of the left amygdala was reduced. Furthermore, while volumes of the left and right amygdala were negatively correlated with a general measure of trait aggression (ie, not one specifically designed to differentiate proactive from reactive aggression), only the left amygdala correlation remained significant after correcting for multiple comparisons. Volumes of neither the left nor right amygdalae were correlated with measures of the interpersonal/affective dimensions of psychopathy. Curiously, an unexpected finding was that right amygdala volumes were found to be negatively correlated with a measure of proactive aggression.

Therefore, in a fairly wide range of populations, an inverse association between amygdala volume and trait aggression appears to be a relatively consistent finding. Further, there is growing evidence that taking the functional subdivisions of the amygdala into account is necessary in order to better resolve the amygdala–aggression relationship. Preliminarily, it appears the left, dorsal amygdala may have a distinct, albeit not exclusive, role. Preferential involvement of the dorsal amygdala points to altered amygdala output functionality; however, it will be necessary to determine the precise nature of this functional effect. Further, involvement of the left amygdala invokes a role for explicit/conscious and effortful mechanisms of affect processing, as opposed to implicit/subconscious and automatic processes, which have been associated with the right amygdala.Reference Dyck, Loughead, Kellermann, Boers, Gur and Mathiak 37

Thus far, we have focused on the relationship between amygdala structure and trait aggression. The functional significance of these structural changes, however, remains unclear. For example, is reduced amygdala volume associated with amygdala hypo- or hyperactivity? Thus, we now turn to functional studies that examine the amygdala’s involvement in aggression.

We recently examined patterns of regional brain metabolic activity in patients comorbid for IED and BPD (N=38) in comparison to healthy control participants (N=36) using [18]fluoro-deoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) in conjunction with a laboratory aggression task, the Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm (PSAP).Reference New, Hazlett and Newmark 38 Importantly, FDG-PET was performed twice, under 2 different PSAP settings: a provoked condition, in which a fictitious competitor would “steal” or subtract points from the participant, and an unprovoked condition, for comparison. As expected, the BPD-IED group exhibited more aggressive behavior during the baseline (unprovoked) condition than the control group; and under the provoked condition, both groups exhibited an increased frequency of aggressive responding toward the (fictitious) competitor relative to the unprovoked state, although the level of aggressive behavior in the BPD-IED group was still greater than that of controls during provocation.

In terms of amygdala metabolic activity, we observed group differences in baseline amygdala activity, as well as in response to provocation: In the unprovoked state, amygdala metabolic activity was lower in the BPD-IED group compared to controls; and in response to provocation, amygdala activity increased in the BPD-IED group and decreased, albeit slightly, in the control group, despite the fact that both groups exhibited a similar degree of increased aggressive responding in the provoked compared to unprovoked condition.Reference New, Hazlett and Newmark 38 Although there are important caveats to interpreting these findings, such as not accounting for amygdala functional subdivisions, they do suggest that amygdala activity is more labile, and perhaps less well modulated, in those with pathological forms of aggression.

A role for enhanced amygdala responsivity in aggression has also been demonstrated in studies employing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). For example, Coccaro et al Reference Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald and Phan 39 previously described enhanced amygdala activity in response to viewing threatening facial expressions (specifically, anger and fear) in aggressive individuals relative to controls. Investigators, therefore, have taken advantage of this paradigm as a means to interrogate the neural structures and circuitry underlying aggression, without having to elicit aggressive behavior in the laboratory setting.

In addition to their analysis of amygdala volume, the study by Bobes et al,Reference Bobes, Ostrosky and Diaz 36 discussed above, also employed fMRI to examine amygdala activation in response to fearful facial expressions. More specifically, they assessed the difference between amygdala responses to fearful and neutral faces (F/N-difference), with the neutral facing serving as a type of control stimulus. They found that in aggressive compared to non-aggressive participants, the F/N-difference was reduced, specifically, in the left dorsal amygdala. Moreover, among all participants, the amygdala F/N-difference was significantly negatively correlated with factors related to impulsive aggression, but not with the interpersonal/affective dimensions of psychopathy.

Such a reduction in the F/N-difference potentially signifies that amygdala responses to fearful faces are reduced in aggressive individuals, which would conflict with the notion of amygdala hyperactivity in aggression. However, Bobes et al Reference Bobes, Ostrosky and Diaz 36 found that amygdala responses to both fearful and neutral faces were in fact increased in the aggressive group relative to the non-aggressive participants; however, the increase for neutral faces was greater than that of fearful faces. Thus, it was the increase in amygdala response to neutral faces that primarily determined the reduced F/N-difference. In other words, it is not that the aggressive group demonstrated less amygdala activity in response to fearful faces compared to the non-aggressive group, but rather, they exhibited greater amygdala activation to both fearful and neutral faces compared to the non-aggressive group. However, the neutral face-difference was even larger than that associated with fearful faces. Thus, the relatively lower F/N-difference in the aggressive group should not be equated with amygdala hypoactivity in response to socially threatening stimuli, but rather, amygdala hyperactivity in response to both fearful and neutral faces, but to a greater extent with respect to the neutral stimuli.

The last functional study we will discuss was performed Lozier et al,Reference Lozier, Cardinale, Vanmeter and Marsh 40 in which they characterized the role of the right amygdala in proactive aggression by examining differences in the processing of fearful facial expression in adolescents with conduct problems (N=30) compared to a healthy adolescent group (N=16). It should be noted that this study differs from the others discussed above in that it consists of an adolescent as opposed to adult population, and is specifically focused on the right amygdala and proactive aggression.

Initially, Lozier et al unexpectedly found an apparent absence of differential right amygdala activity to fearful faces in adolescents with conduct problems compared to controls; however, this was found to be due to “statistical suppressor effects” of associated pathological personality dimensions that exhibited contrasting neural correlates.Reference Lozier, Cardinale, Vanmeter and Marsh 40 Specifically, they found that callous-unemotional (CU) traits (a component of psychopathy) and externalizing behaviors were negatively and positively correlated, respectively, with right amygdala responses to fearful faces. Thus, even though CU traits and externalizing behaviors commonly co-occur, they nevertheless exhibited a contradistinctive relationship with respect to right amygdala response to fearful faces. Such contrasting effects between highly covariant phenomena are a potential source of heterogeneity and inconsistency between studies.

Next, Lozier et al found that, similar to CU traits, proactive aggression was also negatively correlated with amygdala response to fearful faces, while covarying for externalizing traits.Reference Lozier, Cardinale, Vanmeter and Marsh 40 Further, they described that right amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces mediated the relationship between CU traits and proactive aggression. In other words, altered right amygdala functionality may be necessary for CU traits to manifest in terms of proactive aggression. The role of the right amygdala’s differential response to fearful faces serving as link between CU traits and proactive aggression was shown to be specific in 3 ways. First, right amygdala responses to angry or neutral faces did not mediate a link between CU traits and proactive aggression. Second, right amygdala responses to fearful faces did not link CU traits and reactive aggression. And last, right amygdala responses to fearful faces did not link externalizing behaviors and reactive aggression.

Prefrontal cortex and prefrontal-amygdala interactions

Studies of aggression have primarily implicated the limbic PFC, which includes portions of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Generally speaking, the OFC and ACC are both involved in the integration of affective, sensory, and cognitive processes, for the purpose of generating state representations that are coherent, nuanced, and textured, as well as behavioral, cognitive, and affective responses that are modulated, contextually appropriate, and flexible.Reference Walton, Croxson, Behrens, Kennerley and Rushworth 41 , Reference Rudebeck and Murray 42 The OFC is considered to play more of a sensory or perceptual role, eg, appraising the motivational value and affective valence of stimuli; the ACC, on the other hand, is more closely associated with determining “actions” or “responses.”

Surprisingly, only a handful of studies have examined structural differences of the OFC and ACC in aggression. For example, smaller left OFC gray matter volume has been shown to be related to greater levels of trait aggression.Reference Gansler, McLaughlin and Iguchi 43 Similarly, greater right/left OFC volume ratios were also associated with greater trait aggression in those with a history of affective illness.Reference Antonucci, Gansler, Tan, Bhadelia, Patz and Fulwiler 44 In child and adolescent populations, reduced right ACC volumes have been associated with increased aggression.Reference Boes, Tranel, Anderson and Nopoulos 45 , Reference Ducharme, Hudziak and Botteron 46

Early, functional studies indicated an association between aggression and reduced ventromedial PFC basal metabolic activityReference Soloff, Meltzer, Greer, Constantine and Kelly 47 and attenuated metabolic activation to serotonergic agents.Reference New, Hazlett and Buchsbaum 48 A recent study of ours, however, revealed a more complex picture. While metabolic activity of the OFC was lower under control conditions in a BPD-IED group compared to healthy controls, with provocation and increased aggressive responding, the OFC exhibited a relative increase in metabolic activity in the BPD-IED group, whereas in the healthy control group, OFC activity was reduced with provocation and increased aggressive responding.Reference New, Hazlett and Newmark 38 The specificity of this effect for the OFC was demonstrated, as other prefrontal regions (medial, anterior, dorsolateral) did not differ between groups. Finally, we found that the correlation between medial OFC activity and aggressive responding, observed in healthy controls, was absent in the BPD-IED group. These findings suggest that the role of the OFC in the pathophysiology of aggression may not be best ascribed to a “hypoactivity” mechanism, but rather a “disconnection” one. As we will describe below, it has been useful to characterize the neural basis of aggression in terms of differential connectivity or coupling between the ventromedial PFC and the amygdala.

Coccaro et al Reference Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald and Phan 39 described that while viewing angry faces, IED patients (N=10) exhibited increased amygdala and decreased medial OFC activity compared to control participants (N=10). Furthermore, it was described that among controls, amygdala activity was negatively correlated with that of the medial OFC (denoting the 2 regions were functionally coupled), whereas in the aggressive group, no amygdala-OFC coupling was observed.Reference Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald and Phan 39

We will first describe here (and in Figure 2) an overview of the basic neurobiology of amygdala-PFC connectivity before addressing its specific role in aggression. Work in primates has revealed a number of general principles regarding the structural basis of amygdala-PFC interconnectivity. First, the limbic portions of the PFC, namely, the OFC and ACC, are the prefrontal regions that exhibit the most robust reciprocal anatomical connections with the amygdala—particularly, the posterior portion of the OFCReference Ghashghaei, Hilgetag and Barbas 49 , Reference Timbie and Barbas 50 and the pregenual and subgenual (ie, caudal) ACC (Figure 2A).Reference Ghashghaei, Hilgetag and Barbas 49 Amygdala innervation is more scant and unidirectional in the more dorsal and lateral portions of the PFC.Reference Ghashghaei, Hilgetag and Barbas 49 Second, as illustrated in Figure 2A, the ACC sends more projections to the amygdala than it receives, whereas the posterior OFC receives more amygdala projections than it sends. This is consistent with the more “action”- or “response”-oriented functions of the ACC, and the “sensory” or “perceptual” role of the OFC. Finally, the termination patterns of OFC and ACC projections to the amygdala differ (see Figures 2C and 2D for details), which suggests that the former is an arbiter of central nucleus activity, whereas the latter modulates how the basal nucleus processes sensory input.Reference Ghashghaei and Barbas 51

Figure 2 Prefrontal-Amygdala circuitry. Altered prefrontal amygdala circuitry has been implicated in the neurobiology of aggression. (a) Preclinical studies in primates and rodents have demonstrated that the portions of the prefrontal cortex most robustly interconnected, anatomically, with the amygdala include 2 basic regions: the posterior extent of the orbitofrontal cortex (pOFC) and the caudal portions of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), roughly corresponding to the subgenual (sg)ACC and pregenual (pg)ACC. Anatomical connectivity between the amygdala and PFC, in general, decreases as it moves toward the dorsal and lateral aspects of the PFC (not depicted in this figure). Although there is significant bilateral anatomical connectivity between the amygdala and both the ACC and pOFC, studies in primates have also demonstrated relatively greater ACC-to-amygdala projections than amygdala-to-ACC projections; the pOFC, on the other hand, receives a greater number of projections from the amygdala compared to projections the pOFC sends to the amygdala. This has been considered to signify that the ACC is more of a sender of amygdala-PFC projections, whereas, the pOFC is more of a receiver.Reference Ghashghaei, Hilgetag and Barbas 49 , Reference Timbie and Barbas 50 (b) These structural data in primates are broadly consistent with intrinsic functional connectivity studies of the amygdala complex in humans, although the latter provides, of course, relatively less anatomical resolution. As illustrated in (b), the subgenual/orbital portion of the medial PFC exhibits positive functional connectivity (red) with the basolateral amygdala complex, which suggests a predominantly feed-forward relationship. The pregenual medial PFC, on the other hand, exhibits negative functional connectivity with the basolateral amygdala complex (blue), which indicates a feed-back role.Reference Roy, Shehzad and Margulies 52 There is a general absence of functional connectivity with the more dorsal and lateral (not shown) PFC, which is similar to primate structural findings. (c)–(d) There are important anatomical differences in the termination patterns of projections from the OFC and ACC to the amygdala in the primate.Reference Ghashghaei and Barbas 51 Projection neurons from the ACC terminate broadly throughout the basal nucleus (as well as other amygdala nuclei), whereas OFC efferents are relatively unique in that they terminate among the intercalated masses or IMs (clusters of GABAergic inhibitory neurons that regulate central nucleus activity), as well as the central nucleus itself. Therefore, the pOFC may act as a direct arbiter of central nucleus activity, and therefore, determine the extent to which a stimulus representation is infused with a visceral/autonomic or “somatic” component. The ACC, on the other hand, may influence more broadly how sensory inputs to the amygdala are processed. It remains unclear how pregenual and subgenual portions of the ACC may differ in this respect, however.

Human studies of amygdala-PFC connectivity, while unable to provide the same level of neuroanatomical resolution as animal studies, have been broadly consistent with the preclinical findings described above, and have offered additional functional information. As shown in Figure 2B, “resting state” or intrinsic functional connectivity studies have demonstrated that the basolateral nuclear complex is strongly (negatively) coupled with the ventral, pregenual PFC and (positively) coupled with the subgenual/orbital PFC. Also, there is an absence of connectivity between the basolateral complex and the dorsal and lateral portions of the PFC.Reference Roy, Shehzad and Margulies 52 Notably, the negative connectivity between the ventral pregenual PFC and basolateral amygdala is consistent with a primarily “top-down,” inhibitory feed-back process, whereas the positive connectivity with the subgenual/orbital PFC suggests a possible “bottom-up,” feed-forward process.

We will now review specific studies in which aggression has been examined in terms of differential amgydala-PFC connectivity. As described earlier, Coccaro et al Reference Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald and Phan 39 found that the coupling between the amygdala and medial OFC, which occurred while healthy participants viewed angry faces, was absent in patients with IED. Similarly, in a more recent study using resting-state fMRI in patients with schizophrenia (N=25), Hoptman et al Reference Hoptman, D’Angelo and Catalano 53 demonstrated that functional connectivity between the amygdala and ventral PFC was significantly decreased compared to healthy controls (N=21), and critically, they found a significant negative correlation between trait measures of aggression and amygdalo-frontal connectivity. Similarly, studying a nonclinical male population (N=16), Fulwiler et al Reference Fulwiler, King and Zhang 54 observed that trait anger was inversely correlated with functional connectivity between the amygdala and contralateral medial OFC—most strongly between the right amygdala and left medial OFC. Furthermore, they found that a measure of anger control, ie, the propensity to try to suppress one’s response to angry feelings, was positively correlated with amygdala-OFC functional connectivity. An important question for future studies is to what extent altered amygdala-PFC connectivity is structural or neuromodulatory. In a recent study of a large cohort of healthy male participants (N=93), Beyer et al Reference Beyer, Munte, Wiechert, Heldmann and Kramer 55 did not observe a significant difference in a measure of structural connectivity between the amygdala and PFC in those with high compared to low trait-aggressiveness. It will be essential to extend such studies to clinical/pathological manifestations of aggression, however.

In terms of a neuromodulatory basis for altered amygdala-PFC connectivity, the serotonergic system is emerging as a likely candidate.Reference New, Hazlett and Buchsbaum 56 For example, pharmacological antagonism of 5-HT2A receptors enhances medial OFC-amygdala connectivity during the processing of fearful faces.Reference Hornboll, Macoveanu and Rowe 57 As will be discussed further below (in Receptor subtypes: 5-HT2A), this is consistent with our finding that state aggression in personality disordered patients with IED is positively correlated with 5-HT2A receptor availability in the OFC.Reference Rosell, Thompson and Slifstein 58 Dietary tryptophan depletion, which acutely lowers central serotonin synthesis, disrupts amygdala connectivity with the ventral ACC and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, specifically when viewing angry faces.Reference Passamonti, Crockett and Apergis-Schoute 59 Finally, polymorphisms of the 5-HT transporter have been associated with differential amygdala-PFC connectivity.Reference Pezawas, Meyer-Lindenberg and Drabant 60 , Reference Heinz, Braus and Smolka 61

Therefore, it is becoming increasingly clear that while both the amygdala and ventral and medial aspects of the PFC are involved in aggression, it is essential that their concerted function be examined. Further, it is critical that the neuroanatomical specificity of amygdala-PFC interactions be taken into account, as connectivity with the amygdala differs not only between the ACC and OFC, but within functionally distinct subregions of each of these structures.

Striatum

The striatum integrates widespread, direct cortical input, and projects to pallidal structures that ultimately, via direct and indirect pathways, modulate thalamo-cortical activity. Through its involvement in this circuitry, the striatum plays a critical role in the appropriate selection and inhibition of various competing, motor, cognitive, and emotional response sequences.Reference Haber 62

The ventral and dorsomedial portions of the striatum are notable candidates for involvement in aggression, given their role in “goal-directed,” “motivated,” and “value-based” processes. The ventral striatum is believed to be involved in processing the anticipated value of “outcomes” or “events,” whereas the dorsomedial striatum underlies the expected value of “actions.”

Therefore, the ventral striatum conceivably may determine, for example, one’s expectations from social interactions, and therefore contribute to phenomena, such as excessive frustration from interpersonal slights, that are common precipitants of impulsive aggression. The dorsomedial striatum, on the other hand, may be involved in determining the value of certain responses to frustration, such as assaultiveness as opposed to composed restraint.

Furthermore, striatal parameters that determine event and action values are modulated by the dopaminergic and serotonergic systems. Dopamine is believed to encode the expected value of an event or action and also the degree of increase or decrease in synaptic dopamine after an event reflects the extent to which an outcome exceeded or fell short of expectations, respectively. Through its modulation of striatal dopamine, the serotonergic system determines the temporal discount parameter, ie, how much the delay of receiving a reward diminishes its value.

Buckholtz et al Reference Buckholtz, Treadway and Cowan 212 found that in a community sample of adults without a substance abuse history, the impulsive-antisocial dimension of psychopathy was positively correlated with both presynaptic dopamine release-capacity in the ventral striatum, and ventral striatal activity while waiting for a monetary reward. These findings suggest that immoderate ventral striatal function may be associated with impulsive aggression (a component of impulsive antisociality) by leaving an individual susceptible to disproportionate frustration, such as undue sensitivity to interpersonal slights and social rejection, which are common precipitants for aggression.

The dorsomedial striatum, in concert with the serotonergic system, has also been implicated in aggression. Crockett et al Reference Crockett, Apergis-Schoute and Herrmann 63 performed fMRI while participants engaged in a social trust paradigm in which they could accept or reject, and found that dorsomedial striatal activity was directly correlated with a participant “retaliating” by electing to reject an unfair financial offer, despite the fact that this choice was “costly” as both the participant and the offerer would not receive any money. Furthermore, they described that acute lowering of central serotonin levels with dietary tryptophan depletion led participants to reject moderately unfair offers more frequently, and take longer to accept the fairest offers. Activity of the dorsal striatum increased during rejections to a degree commensurate with the increased frequency of rejecting offers.Reference Heinz, Braus and Smolka 61 These findings of an interaction between serotonergic modulation and dorsal striatal activity in punitive, albeit costly, responses to unfair social exchanges is consistent with a finding from a recent study of ours, in which we observed that lower striatal serotonin transporter levels were related to greater state-levels of aggression in personality disordered patients.Reference van de Giessen, Rosell and Thompson 64

Neurochemistry and Pharmacology: Serotonin

Initial studies of serotonin and aggression

One of the best studied areas of the neurobiology of aggression is the role of the serotonergic system. The first evidence for a role for serotonin (5-HT) in aggression came from animal models, in which isolation-induced aggression was shown to be reduced by numerous 5-HTergic antagonists and by pharmacological 5-HT depletion.Reference Malick and Barnett 65 Subsequent evidence from human studies revealed a negative relationship between cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), the major metabolite of 5-HT catabolism, and aggression in patients with personality disorders.Reference Brown, Goodwin, Ballenger, Goyer and Major 66 , Reference Brown, Ebert and Goyer 67 The inverse correlation between 5-HIAA and trait aggression was interpreted as a hypo-serotonergic mechanism of impulsive aggression.

Further early findings demonstrated altered platelet 5-HT parameters, such as decreased 5-HT uptake in aggressive compared to non-aggressive participantsReference Brown, Kent and Bryant 68 and decreased platelet imipramine binding (a measure of 5-HT transporter binding) in aggressive children with conduct disorder.Reference Stoff, Pollock, Vitiello, Behar and Bridger 69 Attenuated hormonal responses to 5-HTergic pharmacological challenges also contributed to the initial foundation of the 5-HT hypothesis of aggression.Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi and Hauger 70 Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi, Cooper and Hauger 72 Finally, the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) fluoxetine was shown to have an anti-aggressive therapeutic effect in personality disordered patients that was independent of antidepressant or anxiolytic effect.Reference Coccaro, Astill, Herbert and Schut 73 , Reference Coccaro and Kavoussi 74

Studies of 5-HT CSF metabolites, platelet measures, hormonal responses to pharmacological challenge, and the anti-aggressive effect of fluoxetine have been repeated and re-examined. There has been significant variability with respect to CSF 5-HIAA levels. While the classic finding consists of an inverse relationship,Reference Brown, Goodwin, Ballenger, Goyer and Major 66 , Reference Brown, Ebert and Goyer 67 , Reference Kruesi, Rapoport and Hamburger 75 at least one study described a positive correlation,Reference Coccaro and Lee 76 whereas another revealed no covariance, despite a reduced fenfluramine-induced prolactin response (and index of presynaptic 5-HT) in the same cohort.Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi, Hauger, Cooper and Ferris 77 While platelet 5-HT concentrationReference Goveas, Csernansky and Coccaro 78 and 5-HT2A receptor levelsReference Coccaro, Kavoussi, Sheline, Berman and Csernansky 79 have also been shown to be related to aggression, reduced platelet 5-HTT binding site number in aggressive patients appears to be the most consistent platelet finding.Reference Marseille, Lee and Coccaro 80 It is worth noting, however, that at least 2 earlier studies found a positive relationship between 5-HTT platelet binding sites and aggressionReference Modai, Gibel, Rauchverger, Ritsner, Klein and Ben-Shachar 81 , Reference Sarne, Mandel, Goncalves, Brook, Gafni and Elizur 82 ; however, these studies were not done in as well-defined an aggressive population, whereas those more consistently demonstrating reductions in platelet 5-HTT have been performed in impulsive aggressive personality disordered patients either with or without a formal diagnosis of IED.

Reduced fenfluramine-induced increases in plasma prolactin also appears to be a highly reliable finding in aggression.Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi and Hauger 70 Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi, Cooper and Hauger 72 , Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi, Hauger, Cooper and Ferris 77 , Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi and Hauger 83 Change in serum prolactin after fenflulramine challenge is negatively related to trait aggression, and is lower in personality disordered patients with the more recent, integrated research criteria for IED (IED-IR); this effect is abrogated when covarying for differences in trait aggression. Moreover, prolactin level changes were not related to impulsivity; a diagnosis of BPD or ASPD; nor mood, anxiety, or alcohol/substance abuse disorders.Reference Coccaro, Lee and Kavoussi 26 Therefore, these findings provided support for a specific involvement of 5-HT in aggression.

While these findings discussed above established the importance of the 5-HT system in aggression, further studies have been essential in more directly demonstrating the ability of the 5-HT system to modulate aggressive behavior, and address questions related to molecular genetics and pharmacology, development, and neuroanatomy. In the following sections, we will discuss the literature related to these issues.

5-HT challenges in humans: laboratory aggression, and functional neuroimaging

One of the primary methods for investigating the role of the 5-HT system in aggression is by the use of peripherally administered agents that acutely attenuate or enhance central 5-HTergic function. One of the most common and well described interventions is dietary depletion of tryptophan, the amino acid precursor of 5-HT biosynthesis, which has been validated as a means to rapidly lower central 5-HT.Reference Carpenter, Anderson and Pelton 84 Reference Williams, Shoaf, Hommer, Rawlings and Linnoila 86

Early studies demonstrated that tryptophan depletion increased aggressive responding in nonclinical male participants in laboratory measures of aggression, which is consistent with the low-presynaptic 5-HT hypothesis.Reference Bjork, Dougherty, Moeller, Cherek and Swann 87 However, it has become increasingly apparent that the relationship between 5-HT and aggression is complex, as a variety of factors appear to moderate how challenges to the 5-HT system affect aggressive responding.

Using a laboratory aggression paradigm, Bjork et al Reference Bjork, Dougherty, Moeller and Swann 88 found that tryptophan depletion and tryptophan loading (which enhances central 5-HT availability) increased and decreased the frequency of aggressive responses, respectively, in those with high trait aggression. In those with low trait aggression, on the other hand, the effect was reversed, with decreases and increases in aggressive responding occurring under tryptophan-depleted and -loaded conditions, respectively.Reference Bjork, Dougherty, Moeller and Swann 88 Similarly, Kramer et al Reference Kramer, Riba, Richter and Munte 89 described an attenuation of aggressive responding with tryptophan depletion in a laboratory aggression paradigm, but only in those with low trait aggression, with no effect on those with high trait aggression levels. In studies of adults and adolescents with ADHD, tryptophan depletion increased aggressive responding in a laboratory paradigm, but only in those with low trait impulsivity.Reference Kotting, Bubenzer, Helmbold, Eisert, Gaber and Zepf 90 Reference Zimmermann, Grabemann and Mette 92

In addition to depletion/loading of dietary of tryptophan, studies have also employed acute administration of SSRIs as a means to temporarily heighten synaptic 5-HT. Two studies have demonstrated that the delivery of severe, retaliatory, (laboratory-based) aggressive responses (which occurs only in those with either high levels of primary psychopathyReference Fanning, Berman, Guillot, Marsic and McCloskey 93 or significant histories of aggression,Reference Berman, McCloskey, Fanning, Schumacher and Coccaro 94 under levels of high-provocation) were reduced, acutely, after a single dose of an SSRI.

A handful of studies have begun to examine the neurobiological mechanisms involved in 5-HTergic modulation of impulse control and emotion processing relevant to aggression. Right orbitofrontal activity during inhibition of a motor response (ie, the no-go condition of a go/no-go task) was reduced after tryptophan depletion.Reference Rubia, Lee and Cleare 95 In a laboratory aggression paradigm, tryptophan depletion was associated with reduced insula activity during selection of severity of aggressive response.Reference Kramer, Riba, Richter and Munte 89 Acute challenges to the serotonergic system have also been shown to influence the processing of emotional facesReference Lee, Gill, Chen, McCloskey and Coccaro 96 and associated neural activity. Tryptophan depletion modulated the functional connectivity that occurs between the amygdala and prefrontal regions specifically while processing angry faces, as opposed to neutral or sad faces.Reference Passamonti, Crockett and Apergis-Schoute 59

Last, Grady et al Reference Grady, Siebner, Hornboll, Macoveanu, Paulson and Knudsen 97 compared the effects of attenuated and enhanced central 5-HT availability (with tryptophan depletion and acute citalopram administration, respectively) on the processing of neutral, angry, and fearful faces. First, they found 2 separate networks—one that was selective for fearful faces relative to angry and neutral expressions, and the other that distinguished between fearful and neutral faces, but with responses to angry faces that was intermediate to those of neutral and fearful ones. In both networks, tryptophan depletion specifically enhanced responses to angry faces, such that they were more like the responses to fear than neutrality. Citalopram treatment, on the other hand, led to neural network responses that were equivalent across the 3 emotions, yet by differing mechanisms. In the first network, increased 5-HT with citalopram decreased responses to fear equal to that of responses to neutrality. In the second network, on the other hand, citalopram increased responses to both neutral and angry faces to be similar to that of fear.

In summary, studies of acute manipulations of 5-HT availability in humans have provided strong support for a modulatory role of aggression by the 5-HTergic system. The mechanism by which 5-HT influences aggression is likely through neural networks involving the amygdala, ventro-medial prefrontal regions, and the striatum. These findings also make clear that the relationship between 5-HT and aggression is not a simple, linear function, and trait dimensions (viz, aggression, impulsivity, psychopathy) and the quality of aggression (normal, pathological, reactive vs. proactive) are important moderating variables. The neural basis of the complex relationship between 5-HT and aggression likely owes to parallel neural circuits that are modulated by 5-HT, yet are differentially involved in the various facets of appraisal and response to social threat-stimuli.

Molecular biology of the 5-HT system in aggression: genetic, pharmacological, and neuroimaging studies

A wide variety of studies, methodologically speaking, have identified various molecular components of the 5-HT system involved in aggression, which can be broadly categorized as being involved in either 5-HT metabolism/turnover or 5-HT receptor signaling. Much of this work has been performed in animal genetic and pharmacological studies; however, there are also data from human genetic studies and neurochemical imaging.

5-HT synthesis and turnover: tryptophan hydroxylase-2

Tryptophan hydroxylase-2 (tph2) was recently found to be the brain-specific, rate-limiting enzyme mediating 5-HT synthesis.Reference Zhang, Beaulieu, Sotnikova, Gainetdinov and Caron 98 The mouse tph2 single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) C1437G has been associated with differential rates of 5-HT synthesis, and was used as a model to examine aggressive behavior, as well as depression and anxiety. Mice homozygous for the high-activity tph2 allele (1437C) were shown to exhibit greater levels of aggression compared to mice homozygous for the low-activity allele (1437G).Reference Osipova, Kulikov and Popova 99 Similarly, in a study in which highly aggressive mice were bred with a less aggressive laboratory breed, midbrain tph2 mRNA was shown to be positively correlated with the extent of severe aggression in certain resultant strains.Reference Takahashi, Shiroishi and Koide 100 While these 2 former studies indicate that greater tph2 activity results in higher levels of aggression, mice with both tph2 copies deleted or “knocked out” (tph2−/−) were found to be more aggressive than tph2(−/+) and tph2(+/+) mice.Reference Mosienko, Bert and Beis 101 Therefore, again, we see that the contribution of the 5-HT system to aggression is complex: “low” tph2 activity and “no” tph2 activity both affect aggression, albeit, in a contrasting manner. This is likely due to differential developmental pathways that occur in the absence of functional protein, and the development of qualitatively different forms of aggression.

To our knowledge, the human equivalent of a null tph2 mutation has not been identified. However, “risk” tph2 polymorphisms have been described in humans and nonhuman primates. Homozygosity for a functional tph2 SNP (associated with altered morning, basal cortisol levels) in rhesus monkeys was associated with attenuated aggressive behavior; however, this effect depended on a gene x environment interaction, as the anti-aggressive effect of this allele occurred only in those animals maternally reared, as opposed to being raised by peers.Reference Chen, Novak, Meyer, Kelly, Vallender and Miller 102 Polymorphisms of tph2 have also been linked to anger-related personality traits;Reference Yang, Lee and Lee 103 , Reference Yoon, Lee, Kim, Lee and Ham 104 Cluster B and C personality disorders; and the personality traits harm-avoidanceReference Gutknecht, Jacob and Strobel 105 and reward dependence.Reference Inoue, Yamasue and Tochigi 106 Our group recently found that a risk tph2 haplotype occurred with greater frequency in patients with BPD compared to healthy controls, and BPD patients with the risk haplotype exhibited greater levels of aggression, as well as affective lability and suicidal/parasuicidal symptoms.Reference Perez-Rodriguez, Weinstein and New 107

Polymorphisms of tph2 have also been related to structural and functional neuroanatomical differences related to aggression. Differential bilateral, dorsal amygdala reactivity to processing of angry/fearful faces (a putative biomarker of aggression) was shown to be related to tph2 polymorphism genotype.Reference Brown, Peet and Manuck 108 In vivo 5-HT synthesis capacity, specifically, in the medial OFC—a brain region that plays a key role in aggression—has been shown to be related to tph2 polymorphisms in healthy, human control participants.Reference Booij, Turecki and Leyton 109 Moreover, carriers of a risk tph2 allele had larger medial OFC volumes,Reference Takahashi, Shiroishi and Koide 100 as well as reduced amygdala and hippocampal volumes.Reference Chen, Novak, Meyer, Kelly, Vallender and Miller 102

5-HT synthesis and turnover: 5-HT transporter

The 5-HT transporter (5-HTT) is distributed along the axons and synaptic terminals of 5-HTergic neurons and plays an essential role in the clearance of extracellular 5-HT. There is a considerable mouse and human genetics literature on the role of the 5-HTT in aggression. Attenuated aggression, as well as increased anxiety and stress-responses, were observed in mice with both 5-HTT genes knocked out (KO).Reference Heiming, Monning, Jansen, Kloke, Lesch and Sachser 110 , Reference Holmes, Murphy and Crawley 111 Importantly, there appear to be interactions between genotype and the context of aggression. Homozygous 5-HTT KO mice were less aggressive than wild-type and heterozygous KO mice, specifically when in the opponent’s environment as opposed to either a neutral or home setting. Heterozygous 5-HTT KO mice, on the other hand, were more aggressive than wild-type mice in their home environments, but no different than wild-type mice in a neutral setting or that of the opponent. Aggression was not affected by venue (home, neutral, or opponent’s) in the wild-type. Last, no differences in aggression were found among the 3 genotypes in a neutral environment.Reference Heiming, Monning, Jansen, Kloke, Lesch and Sachser 110

Mouse studies of genetically deleted 5-HTT have been complemented by those employing pharmacological antagonism of the 5-HTT (eg, with fluoxetine treatment), which has allowed for the isolation of specific developmental periods. Interestingly, aggression in mature mice is reduced when mice are treated with fluoxetine during either an early postnatal period or a peri-adolescent one; increases in anxiety in the mature mouse, however, occur with fluoxetine treatment only during the early postnatal period, but not peri-adolescence.Reference Yu, Teixeira and Mahadevia 112 Therefore, the developmental roles of 5-HTT in aggression and anxiety are not necessarily fully interdependent, particularly during the critical peri-adolescent period. Surprisingly, it was demonstrated that male offspring of mice treated with fluoxetine while pregnant (ie, in utero 5-HTT inhibition) exhibited increased aggression and reduced anxiety,Reference Kiryanova and Dyck 113 which, remarkably, is the opposite effect of postnatal fluoxetine treatment. Therefore, in addition to critical, early postnatal, and peri-adolescent developmental periods, in which 5-HTT antagonism lessens aggression in the adult, a separate, prenatal period may exist during which 5-HTT antagonism leads to increased aggression in the mature mouse.

Human genetic studies have also examined the role of the 5-HTT in aggression. The most common 5-HTT gene variant consists of the “short” and “long” forms of the 5-HTT promoter region (conventionally referred to as the 5-HTT-linked polymorphic region or 5-HTTLPR), which are associated with relatively decreased and increased expression, respectively.Reference Heils, Teufel and Petri 114 Although at least one study, of adult males with intellectual disabilities, demonstrated an association between the long 5-HTTLPR allele and greater levels of aggression,Reference May, Lightfoot, Srour, Kowalchuk and Kennedy 115 the short allele has more consistently been shown to confer risk for aggression and violence in a range of populations.Reference Hallikainen, Saito and Lachman 116 Reference Beitchman, Baldassarra and Mik 118 Furthermore, it appears that the contribution of the short 5-HTTLPR allele to increased aggression is moderated by environmental factors, such as acute stress,Reference Verona, Joiner, Johnson and Bender 119 recent, chronic stress,Reference Conway, Keenan-Miller, Hammen, Lind, Najman and Brennan 120 and childhood adversity.Reference Reif, Rosler and Freitag 121

In addition to the 5-HTTLPR, another risk polymorphism consists of a variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) within intron 2 of the 5-HTT gene (STin2). The 2 major variants of STin2 consist of a 10- and 12-repeat, with the latter being the risk allele. Two studies have demonstrated that the contribution of risk by the 5-HTT gene for disinhibited/impulsive behaviorReference Aluja, Garcia, Blanch, De and Fibla 122 and aggressionReference Payer, Nurmi, Wilson, McCracken and London 123 was most discernible when both the 5-HTTLPR and STin2 polymorphisms were taken into account (namely, at least 1 short 5-HTTLPR allele and 2 STin2 12-repeat alleles).

In addition to assessing genotype, studies have begun to characterize the epigenetic status of the 5-HTT gene. Generally speaking, such studies have demonstrated a relationship between differential 5-HTT gene methylation and 5-HTT mRNA levels,Reference Philibert, Madan, Andersen, Cadoret, Packer and Sandhu 124 vulnerability to PTSD,Reference Koenen, Uddin and Chang 125 differences in depressive symptoms between monozygotic twins,Reference Zhao, Goldberg, Bremner and Vaccarino 126 hippocampal gray matter volume,Reference Dannlowski, Kugel and Redlich 127 and amygdala threat-related activity.Reference Nikolova, Koenen and Galea 128 At least one study has demonstrated greater 5-HTT gene methylation in adults with a history of childhood aggression compared to adults without such a history; furthermore, in vivo brain 5-HT synthesis in the OFC was negatively correlated with 5-HTT gene methylation.Reference Wang, Szyf and Benkelfat 129

Neurochemical imaging studies, ie, those employing PET or SPECT, have also been performed with radioligands with high affinity for 5-HTT. An early PET study of ours revealed lower 5-HTT binding in the left ACC in impulsive-aggressive personality disordered patients,Reference Frankle, Lombardo and New 130 which suggests attenuated presynaptic 5-HT in fronto-limbic regions and, therefore, is consistent with the hyposerotonergic model of impulsive aggression. In a more recent study, employing a more specific radioligand and larger cohorts of personality disordered IED patients and healthy controls we did not observe the predicted inverse relationship between ACC 5-HTT availability and trait aggression until we statistically adjusted for trait callousness (a measure of the interpersonal/affective component of psychopathy), which itself was positively correlated with ACC 5-HTT availability.Reference van de Giessen, Rosell and Thompson 64

5-HT synthesis and turnover: MAOA

Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) is a catabolic enzyme present in the outer mitochondrial membrane of synaptic terminals of monoaminergic projection neurons; its major neurochemical substrates are 5-HT, NE, and to a lesser extent DA.Reference Bortolato, Chen and Shih 131 MAOA was one of the first genes identified as playing a role in the heritability of impulsive aggression, as a rare missense mutation was identified in a Dutch kindred with a significant history of impulsive aggression.Reference Brunner, Nelen, Breakefield, Ropers and van Oost 132 A more common human polymorphism of MAOA, consisting of a VNTR in the promoter region, has also been well characterized.Reference Sabol, Hu and Hamer 133 Carriers of the low-expressing MAOA-VNTR allele (3 repeats) exhibit greater aggression compared to those with the high-expressing allele (3.5 and 4 repeats),Reference Kuepper, Grant, Wielpuetz and Hennig 134 , Reference Manuck, Flory, Ferrell, Mann and Muldoon 135 and the low-expressing allele occurs more commonly in violent compared to nonviolent incarcerated males.Reference Stetler, Davis and Leavitt 136 Moreover, numerous studies have demonstrated a gene x environment (GxE) effect, such that greater early-life stress increases the risk for impulsive aggression in males with the low-expressing MAOA-VNTR allele (note that this effect is most robust in men, as MAOA is an X-linked gene).Reference Byrd and Manuck 137 This GxE effect has also been demonstrated in nonhuman primates.Reference Karere, Kinnally, Sanchez, Famula, Lyons and Capitanio 138 , Reference Newman, Syagailo and Barr 139 Finally, using PET imaging with the MAOA radioligand [11C]clorgyline, trait aggression in a healthy, nonclinical male population was negatively associated with brain MAOA availability in cortical and subcortical regions; furthermore, this effect was independent of MAOA-VNTR genotype.Reference Alia-Klein, Goldstein and Kriplani 140

Mouse genetic and pharmacological studies have helped to clarify the pathogenesis of MAOA-associated aggression. Increased aggression has been observed in both MAOA knockout miceReference Cases, Seif and Grimsby 141 and mice with a naturally occurring mutationReference Scott, Bortolato, Chen and Shih 142 that very closely mimics the rare missense mutation of the Dutch kindred (resulting, effectively, in a naturally occurring knockout). Early studies demonstrated that brain 5-HT levels, compared to NE and DA, are preferentially increased in the early postnatal phase of mice with MAOA gene deletions, thus suggesting that the increased aggression associated with loss or attenuation of MAOA activity was 5-HT–dependent.Reference Cases, Seif and Grimsby 141 However, while inhibition of 5-HT synthesis in the early postnatal period of MAOA deficient mice has been shown to abrogate certain neuroanatomical and behavioral changes,Reference Cases, Vitalis, Seif, De Maeyer, Sotelo and Gaspar 143 the development of heightened aggression, nevertheless, is unchanged.Reference Bortolato, Godar and Tambaro 144 Furthermore, inhibition of the dopamine transporter, but not the 5-HTT or the norepinephrine transporter, in developing mice leads to an aggressive phenotype comparable to that of MAOA deficient mice.Reference Yu, Teixeira and Mahadevia 112 Therefore, a question for future studies will be to clarify the role of excessive 5-HT vs DA in the pathogenesis of MAOA-dependent aggression.

Receptor subtypes: 5-HT1A

The 5-HT1A receptor plays a clear role in mood and anxiety disorders,Reference Garcia-Garcia, Newman-Tancredi and Leonardo 145 and there is a growing literature examining its role in aggression, as well as the closely related construct of impulsivity. The 5-HT1A receptor is expressed on 2 neuronal populations: (1) as an autoreceptor, in a somato-dendritic distribution, on raphe serotonergic neurons; and (2) postsynaptically, on non-serotonergic hippocampal, septal, and cortical neurons. In both populations, the 5-HT1A receptor promotes membrane hyperpolarization, and therefore, mediates an inhibitory action on target cells.Reference Polter and Li 146

In various rodent models, pharmacological treatment with 5-HT1A agonists attenuates aggressive behaviorsReference Miczek, Hussain and Faccidomo 147 , Reference Pruus, Skrebuhhova-Malmros, Rudissaar, Matto and Allikmets 148 ; there is evidence for the importance of actions at both raphe autoreceptors and frontal postsynaptic receptors.Reference Centenaro, Vieira, Zimmermann, Miczek, Lucion and de Almeida 149 Reference Stein, Miczek, Lucion and de Almeida 152 There is differential 5-HT1A receptor expression and 5-HT1A-dependent functional responses in rodent models of aggression.Reference Naumenko, Kozhemyakina, Plyusnina, Kulikov and Popova 153 Reference van der Vegt, de Boer, Buwalda, de Ruiter, de Jong and Koolhaas 156 In 5-HT1A receptor knockout mice, there is little to no evidence of altered aggression, despite a clear effect on measures related to mood and anxiety conditions. On the other hand, however, mice overexpressing the 5-HT1A receptor (specifically in raphe, 5-HTergic neurons) exhibited heightened aggression.Reference Audero, Mlinar, Baccini, Skachokova, Corradetti and Gross 157 Importantly, this effect was dependent on overexpression of the 5-HT1A receptor during adulthood rather than developmentally.

In humans, agents with significant 5-HT1A receptor activity, eg, buspirone, have not played a significant role in the pharmacotherapy of aggression and violence. Nevertheless, a consistent finding has been an inverse relationship between 5-HT1A receptor-dependent increases in serum cortisol and trait aggression in personality disordered patients.Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi and Hauger 70 , Reference Almeida, Lee and Coccaro 158 , Reference Faccidomo, Quadros, Takahashi, Fish and Miczek 169 In healthy participants, however, 5-HT1A receptor-dependent increases in serum cortisol were positively correlated with trait aggression, suggesting differential involvement of 5-HT1A receptor function in pathologic and “normal” forms of aggression. Polymorphisms of the 5-HT1A receptor gene have not been shown to be involved in impulsive aggression; however, they have been implicated in trait impulsivityReference Benko, Lazary and Molnar 160 and BPD.Reference Joyce, Stephenson, Kennedy, Mulder and McHugh 161 Curiously, 2 PET studies, both using the same 5-HT1A receptor radioligand ([11C]WAY100635) in healthy participants, demonstrated the opposite relation between frontal 5-HT1A receptor availability and trait aggression. One showed a positiveReference Witte, Floel and Stein 162 and the other an inverse correlation.Reference Parsey, Oquendo and Simpson 163 We speculate that the role of the 5-HT1A receptor in aggression may be due to pathophysiologic heterogeneity.

Receptor subtypes: 5-HT1B

The 5-HT1B receptor is expressed in raphe 5-HTergic neurons, as well as non-5-HTergic (viz, GABAergic and glutamatergic) projection neurons originating from the striatum and hippocampus (namely, CA1 pyramidal cells). The receptors themselves are localized primarily at axonal terminals, and are believed to regulate neurotransmitter release.Reference Sari 164 Multiple rodent studies have demonstrated the ability of 5-HT1B agonists to mediate an anti-aggressive effect; however, which neuronal populations are responsible for this effect remains a matter of debate.Reference da Veiga, Miczek, Lucion and de Almeida 150 , Reference Bannai, Fish, Faccidomo and Miczek 165 Reference Veiga, Miczek, Lucion and Almeida 172 Mice with both copies of the 5-HT1B receptor gene knocked out have been shown to exhibit enhanced aggressionReference Ramboz, Saudou and Amara 173 as well as greater impulsivity.Reference Bouwknecht, Hijzen, van der Gugten, Maes, Hen and Olivier 174 In humans, evidence for a role of the 5-HT1B receptor in aggression has been limited to genetic polymorphisms.Reference Conner, Jensen, Tennen, Furneaux, Kranzler and Covault 175 Reference Zouk, McGirr, Lebel, Benkelfat, Rouleau and Turecki 178 No PET imaging studies have been performed examining 5-HT1B receptor availability in aggression; however, such studies have been performed in externalizing or impulse control disorders, such as pathological gambling and alcohol dependence. In pathological gambling, 5-HT1B availability in the striatum and anterior cingulate was positively correlated with severity of gambling,Reference Potenza, Walderhaug and Henry 179 and 5-HT1B availability was greater in the ventral striatum in patients with alcohol dependence compared to controls.Reference Hu, Henry and Gallezot 180 In major depression, on the other hand, 5-HT1B binding is decreased in the ventral striatum compared to controls.Reference Murrough, Henry and Hu 181 Thus, there appear to be contrasting differences in 5-HT1B availability in impulse control disorders (increased 5-HT1B) compared to an “internalizing” condition (decreased 5-HT1B) such as major depression.

Receptor subtypes: 5-HT2A

Consistent with the important anti-aggressive and mood stabilizing effects of agents with 5-HT2A receptor antagonist activity, the 5-HT2A receptor has also been the focus of studies examining the etiology, pathogenesis, and pathophysiology of impulsive aggression. In animal pharmacological studies, the 5-HT2A receptor has been shown to promote impulsive and aggressive behavior.Reference Higgins, Enderlin, Haman and Fletcher 182 Reference Winstanley, Theobald, Dalley, Glennon and Robbins 184 A number of human genetic studies have revealed an association between 5-HT2A receptor polymorphisms and aggressionReference Giegling, Hartmann, Moller and Rujescu 185 and impulse control disorders.Reference Bruce, Steiger, Joober, Ng Ying Kin, Israel and Young 186 Reference Bjork, Moeller, Dougherty, Swann, Machado and Hanis 191 In a postmortem study of suicide victims, prefrontal 5-HT2A receptor binding was positively correlated with lifetime history of aggression.Reference Oquendo, Russo and Underwood 192

A number of PET imaging studies have revealed an association between the 5-HT2A receptor and aggression and impulsivity; however, the results have been conflicting. We recently found that 5-HT2A receptor availability in the OFC was positively associated with state levels of aggression in a physically aggressive personality disordered (predominantly BPD) population meeting criteria for IED.Reference Rosell, Thompson and Slifstein 58 This finding was consistent with our view that OFC 5-HT2A receptor activity promotes aggression, in a dynamic, state-dependent manner, in those with IED. We suspect that increased OFC 5-HT2A receptor availability may be due to psychosocial stress,Reference Dwivedi, Mondal, Payappagoudar and Rizavi 193 and that greater 5-HT2A receptor function in the OFC may promote aggression by decreasing amygdala-OFC coupling that occurs during threat processing.Reference Hornboll, Macoveanu and Rowe 57 At least 2 other studies found, however, that frontal 5-HT2A receptor availability was negatively correlated with trait aggressionReference Soloff, Chiappetta, Mason, Becker and Price 194 in patients with BPD, and impulsivityReference Meyer, Wilson and Rusjan 195 in a highly assaultive population with ASPD. Yet another study found no differences in cortical 5-HT2A receptor levels between high- and low-aggressive participants, as well as no relation to trait aggression or impulsivity.Reference Rylands, Hinz and Jones 196 These studies did not use a measure of state aggression and did not assess for whether participants met criteria for IED; therefore, it is difficult to compare these findings with our study. However, we suspect that the negative correlation between trait aggression and impulsivity and cortical 5-HT2A receptor availability may reflect a compensatory, albeit incomplete, downregulation of the 5-HT2A receptor in aggressive/impulsive individuals.

Receptor subtypes: 5-HT3

The 5-HT3 receptor is the only 5-HT receptor that functions as a ligand-gated ion channel, whereas all other known 5-HT receptors are G-protein coupled. The 5-HT3 receptor has both a pre- and postsynaptic distribution, and in the forebrain is expressed at least in part by a subpopulation of interneurons.Reference Chameau and van Hooft 197 One 5-HT3–dependent mechanism of particular relevance to impulsivity and aggression is the regulation of mesolimbic dopamine release,Reference De Deurwaerdère, Moison, Navailles, Porras and Spampinato 198 which plays a key role in motivation and reward. The possibility that the 5-HT3 receptor is involved in impulsive aggression is limited to animal pharmacological studies, in which 5-HT3 receptor antagonists and agonists led to attenuated and increased aggressive behavior, respectively.Reference Cervantes and Delville 199 Reference McKenzie-Quirk, Girasa, Allan and Miczek 203 To our knowledge, no clinical studies have examined 5-HT3 receptor antagonists in aggression. In alcohol use disorders, however, 5-HT3 antagonism has been shown to lessen alcohol consumption,Reference Sellers, Toneatto, Romach, Somer, Sobell and Sobell 204 lessen subsyndromal mood symptoms,Reference Johnson, Ait-Daoud, Ma and Wang 205 and attenuate alcohol cue-induced dopamine release in the ventral striatum.Reference Myrick, Anton, Li, Henderson, Randall and Voronin 206 Genetic polymorphisms of the 5-HT3 receptor have been associated with impulse control disorders, such as comorbid alcoholism and ASPD,Reference Ducci, Enoch and Yuan 207 and the pathological personality traits of harm avoidance and nonconformity.Reference Melke, Westberg and Nilsson 208 Furthermore, 5-HT3 polymorphisms are associated with differential structure and function of limbic and prefrontal regions.Reference Gatt, Williams and Schofield 209 , Reference Iidaka, Ozaki and Matsumoto 210 Future studies are needed to examine 5-HT3 antagonists clinically in aggressive populations, however.

Neurochemistry and Pharmacology: Dopamine

In contrast to the serotonergic system, much less has been described regarding the role of the dopaminergic (DAergic) system in aggression. Nevertheless, the DAergic system is poised to play an important role in aggression, given its involvement in decision making, reward, motivation, and higher-order cognitive processes. Additionally, the DAergic system is involved in the pathophysiology and psychopharmacology of conditions such as ADHD and schizophrenia, which commonly involve aggressive behavior.

A recent study by Schlüter et al Reference Walters 211 is one of the first to use neurochemical imaging to specifically examine the role of the DAergic system in aggression in humans. Dopamine synthesis and storage capacity were assessed with PET imaging using [(18)F]DOPA in a cohort of healthy, nonclinical men who engaged in a computer-based laboratory task designed to assess aggressive reactions in response to provocation by an unseen and fictitious counterpart.

It was found that the frequency of aggressive responses by participants was inversely correlated with DA synthesis capacity, most notably in the midbrain, but also in the striatum. Additionally, DA storage capacity in the midbrain was negatively correlated with aggressive responses. These findings suggest that greater availability of dopamine may protect against non-advantageous, aggressive responses to provocation. Important questions that remain are (1) how do these findings relate to pathological forms of aggression occurring in a “real world” or non-laboratory setting? and (2) which “downstream,” DA-modulated neuroanatomical regions are being affected and leading to differential aggressive responses?

As studies of dopaminergic neurochemistry and aggression are currently limited, comparison with findings of related constructs, such as psychopathy, may also be informative. Buckholtz et al Reference Buckholtz, Treadway and Cowan 212 assessed presynaptic DA release in a community sample of adults without a history of substance abuse, who were characterized with multiple self-report measures of various personality dimensions, including the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI). Presynaptic DA release in the nucleus accumbens, a striatal region that plays a critical role in reward, was found to be positively correlated with the “impulsive antisocial” component of the PPI; impulsive antisociality is related to, but is somewhat broader than, impulsive aggression. No relation between accumbal presynaptic dopamine was found with the “fearless dominance” component of the PPI, which consists of fearlessness, social influence, and stress immunity. Moreover, the relation between presynaptic DA release and impulsive antisociality remained significant even after adjusting for measures of impulsivity, extraversion, and novelty seeking.

Thus, there is an apparent conflict between the studies by Schlüter et al Reference Schlüter, Winz and Henkel 211 and Buckholtz et al,Reference Buckholtz, Treadway and Cowan 212 with the former indicating a negative relationship between presynaptic DA and aggression and the latter suggesting the opposite. We suspect that the findings of the Schlüter et al. study may reflect the cognitive enhancing effects of adequate DAergic availability on fronto-cortical systems, which may mitigate maladaptive, aggressive responses to frustration. The increased presynaptic DA release in the ventral striatum observed in the Buckholtz et al study, however, may reflect aberrant regulation at synaptic DA terminals in the ventral striatum, and therefore impairment of reward-related processes.

A somewhat broader literature exists regarding the molecular genetic basis of the role of the dopaminergic system in aggression. A number of studies have examined the role of catcehol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), which is involved in the catabolism of DA as well as other catecholamines such as norepinephrine. The Val158Met polymorphism is a common and well-studied variant of COMT: the Met allele is the lower activity variant, resulting in higher basal levels of synaptic dopamine. Interestingly, in patients with BPD, a history of childhood sexual abuse was associated with lower levels of trait aggression, but only in those homozygous for the higher activity, Val158 COMT allele.Reference Wagner, Baskaya, Anicker, Dahmen, Lieb and Tadic 213 This suggests that early psychosocial adversity in those with low basal DA availability may attenuate trait aggression, at least in those predisposed to develop BPD. In patients with schizophrenia, the low activity, Met158 COMT allele has been shown to increase the risk of aggression and violence.Reference Bhakta, Zhang and Malhotra 214 While not all studies investigating the Val158Met COMT polymorphism have demonstrated its involvement in aggression,Reference Flory, Xu, New, Finch, Goldman and Siever 215 Reference Vevera, Stopkova and Bes 217 less well described COMT variants have been implicated in violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder,Reference Vevera, Stopkova and Bes 217 aggressive personality disordered patients,Reference Flory, Xu, New, Finch, Goldman and Siever 215 and highly aggressive children.Reference Hirata, Zai, Nowrouzi, Beitchman and Kennedy 218 Questions for future studies are (a) to what extent is differential COMT activity involved in aggression during development as opposed to adult brain function? and (b) which catecholaminergic substrate of COMT is predominantly involved in determining aggression—DA, or others, such as norepinephrine?

At least 2 studies have demonstrated a role for the 48 bp repeat exon III polymorphism of the dopamine D4 receptor in aggression. The 7-repeat (7R) D4 polymorphism was found to be significantly more common in aggressive patients with schizophrenia, compared to non-aggressive schizophrenia patients and healthy controls.Reference Fresan, Camarena, Apiquian, Aguilar, Urraca and Nicolini 219 Moreover, D4 7R allele carriers exhibited significantly greater “physical aggression against others” compared to the non-7R carriers; however, no differences in 7R allele frequency were observed with respect to other forms of aggression, namely “verbal aggression,” “self-directed aggression,” and “physical aggression against objects.” Such a finding suggests the possibility, for example, that the D4 receptor could be related to proactive aggression, which consists of greater physical assaultiveness. In a separate study, a significant gene x environment interaction was found with the D4 receptor, such that increasing levels of prenatal stress were associated with higher levels of trait aggression manifested during young adulthood only in those with the 7R D4 allele.Reference Buchmann, Zohsel and Blomeyer 220 Neither prenatal stress nor the 7R D4 allele alone was related to trait aggression during young adulthood. Moreover, this effect was specific for prenatal stress, as maternal stress up until the age of 15 of participants was not related to aggression, alone or as a function of D4 allele frequency.

These findings indicate that the DAergic system, in terms of gene x environment interactions, may be involved during development in determining an individual’s propensity for aggression. Issues to be addressed in future studies include (a) whether DAergic agents, such as those targeting the D4 receptor, might have anti-aggressive therapeutic potential in the adult and (b) whether aggression mediated by the DAergic system differs qualitatively from aggression related to 5-HTergic abnormalities.

Neurochemistry and Pharmacology: Vasopressin

While the arginine vasopressin (AVP) system has been well characterized in terms of its neuroendocrine role in the cardiovascular and renal systems, a significant number of animal studies have revealed its role as a direct neuromodulator in the CNS that regulates various social behaviors, such as aggression. Early studies in the hamster demonstrated that direct, hypothalamic administration of AVP enhanced aggressive responses,Reference Delville, Mansour and Ferris 221 , Reference Ferris, Melloni, Koppel, Perry, Fuller and Delville 222 whereas AVP antagonists attenuated them.Reference Ferris and Potegal 223 More recent studies in a rat model of maternal aggression revealed that AVP release in the central nucleus of the amygdala was increased during maternal aggression, and the degree of AVP release was positively correlated with the degree of aggressive behavior. Furthermore, bilateral central amygdala administration of a vasopressin 1a receptor antagonist attenuated maternal aggression, and in a related rat strain with low levels of maternal aggression, AVP administration into the central amygdala nucleus increased aggression.Reference Bosch and Neumann 224

In mouse genetic models, knockout of the AVP 1b receptor reduced aggression,Reference Wersinger, Caldwell, Christiansen and Young 225 , Reference Wersinger, Ginns, O’Carroll, Lolait and Young 226 whereas deletion of the 1a receptor did not,Reference Wersinger, Caldwell, Martinez, Gold, Hu and Young 227 despite the fact that pharmacological manipulations at the 1a receptor significantly influenced aggression and other social behaviors in animal models. A rat line with a naturally occurring deletion of the vasopressin gene also demonstrated decreased aggression and impulsivity.Reference Fodor, Barsvari and Aliczki 228

Human studies have also lent support to the role of the AVP system in impulsive aggression. In a study of impulsive aggressive personality disordered patients, AVP obtained from the CSF was positively correlated with trait aggression, and negatively correlated with a measure of central 5-HTergic activity (ie, fenfluramine-induced serum prolactin levels).Reference Coccaro, Kavoussi, Hauger, Cooper and Ferris 77 The positive relation between CSF AVP and trait aggression persisted after covarying for fenfluramine-induced serum prolactin (an index of 5-HT activity), indicating that the relationship between AVP and aggression cannot simply be accounted for by the relationship between AVP and 5-HT. Intranasal administration of AVP has been shown to affect emotion processing in humans,Reference Uzefovsky, Shalev, Israel, Knafo and Ebstein 229 , Reference Guastella, Kenyon, Alvares, Carson and Hickie 230 and the orally available AVP V1a receptor antagonist, SRX246, was shown to block the effect of intranasal AVP on the neural responses to angry faces in the right amygdala and various other cortical regions associated with social information processing.Reference Lee, Coccaro and Cremers 231 Last, a handful of human genetic studies have suggested that AVP receptor polymorphisms are associated with human aggression, particularly, the 1b receptor gene and childhood aggression.Reference Luppino, Moul, Hawes, Brennan and Dadds 232 Reference Vogel, Wagner and Baskaya 234

Therefore, both genetic and pharmacological evidence in animal models and humans suggests that AVP may promote aggression in regions such as the central amygdala, and this effect may be mediated by the V1a and/or V1b receptors. Furthermore, pharmacological treatments that target the AVP system may be a novel therapeutic modality for aggressive behaviors.

Steroid Hormones: Cortisol and Testosterone

We will examine the steroid hormones cortisol and testosterone together, as there is growing evidence that these 2 steroid hormones may contribute to aggression and violence in an interdependent manner. Initial studies, of male adolescents with disruptive/delinquent behaviors, revealed a relationship between low baseline cortisol and heightened trait aggression.Reference McBurnett, Lahey, Rathouz and Loeber 235 Subsequently, it was found that a positive correlation between serum testosterone levels and aggression depended on the presence of low serum cortisol.Reference Popma, Vermeiren and Geluk 236 Thus, the effect of testosterone on aggression appeared to be moderated by serum cortisol levels.

More recent studies have revealed that the interaction between cortisol and testosterone may be more complex than a “high testosterone/low cortisol” model, and we suspect this may be related to gender and whether participants are either healthy/nonclinical, or are highly aggressive and/or exhibit psychopathic traits. For example, in nonclinical participants, trait aggression was positively associated, only in men, in those with either (a) high levels of testosterone in conjunction with high SSRI-induced cortisol or (b) low testosterone and low SSRI-induced cortisol.Reference Kuepper, Alexander and Osinsky 237 In a separate study, also in nonclinical participants, subclinical psychopathic traits was positively related to testosterone levels in those with high cortisol, and conversely, in those with low cortisol, subclinical psychopathy was negatively related to testosterone levels; both of these effects were only observed in the male participants.Reference Welker, Lozoya, Campbell, Neumann and Carre 238 In a study of a nonclinical group of women, aggressive responses after provocation in a laboratory setting were greater either in those with high testosterone/high cortisol or low testosterone/low cortisol.Reference Denson, Mehta and Ho 239 Last, within a criminal population, morning cortisol levels were negatively correlated with psychopathy scores in psychopathic criminals, but not nonpsychopathic ones.Reference Cima, Smeets and Jelicic 240 Therefore, while testosterone and cortisol appear to be inter-related with respect to aggression, the nature of this relationship likely differs as a function of variables such as gender, psychopathy, and other pathological personality dimensions.

Studies have begun to reveal potential neurobiological mechanisms that underlie the effect of cortisol and testosterone on aggression. Derntl et al Reference Derntl, Windischberger and Robinson 241 demonstrated that reaction times to recognizing fearful male faces, specifically, was negatively related to morning testosterone levels in a nonclinical male population. In other words, greater testosterone levels were associated with recognizing fearful male faces more quickly. Notably, no relationship between testosterone and reaction times to recognizing other emotional facial expressions was observed,Reference Derntl, Windischberger and Robinson 241 indicating that this effect was specific to the fear-processing circuitry associated with aggression, rather than a more global effect on facial affect processing. Amygdala responses to angry and fearful facial expressions, specifically, were directly correlated with testosterone levels,Reference Derntl, Windischberger and Robinson 241 thus implicating the amygdala fear-processing circuitry as a potential substrate of the aggression-promoting effects of testosterone.

Hermans et al Reference Hermans, Ramsey and van Honk 242 found that in a group of nonclinical, female participants, the degree of activation of the amygdala, hypothalamus, and brainstem while viewing angry faces was directly related to the baseline testosterone-to-cortisol (T/C) ratio. Interestingly, while the lateral OFC was also activated in response to angry faces in this study, its activity was not related to the T/C ratio, indicating that contribution of these steroid hormones may act on specific anger-associated brain circuits. In a nonclinical group of women, van Wingen et al Reference van Wingen, Mattern, Verkes, Buitelaar and Fernández 243 found that intranasal testosterone administration reduced functional coupling of the left amygdala with the left OFC and with the right amygdala while performing a facial emotional processing task, indicating that testosterone may modulate amygdala-PFC circuitry involved in social threat processing. Finally, exogenous testosterone administration, after first normalizing endogenous testosterone with treatment with a gonadotropin releasing hormone antagonist in a group of nonclinical males, was associated with increased amygdala, hypothalamus, and periaqueductal gray activity, compared to placebo, while viewing angry facial expressions.Reference Goetz, Tang, Thomason, Diamond, Hariri and Carre 244

Collectively, these findings suggest that cortisol and testosterone influence aggression and related factors such as psychopathy in an interdependent manner; however, this relationship is complex, and likely depends on factors such as age, gender, and degree of trait aggression and psychopathy. Cortisol and testosterone likely influence aggression and psychopathy through the modulation of amygdala-PFC fear or threat circuitry. Areas of focus for future studies consist of identifying the precise loci of action of these steroid hormones, as well as the underlying cellular, molecular, and physiologic effects.

Conclusion

The studies we examined in this review have enhanced our understanding of the neurobiology of aggression and violence, but have also revealed areas of inconsistency that require clarification, as well as clues for avenues of future inquiries.

A relatively consistent and specific finding has been an association between reduced amygdala volumes and greater levels of trait aggression. Optimal resolution of this relationship appears to require analysis of a number of additional variables, such as amygdala subregion and hemisphere, aggression subtype (reactive vs proactive), and covariant dimensions (viz, impulsivity, emotional reactivity, and psychopathy). Inconsistencies between studies—such as some demonstrating reduced whole amygdala volumes, bilaterally, and others indicating a specific involvement of the left dorsal amygdala—likely are resolvable by accounting for these anatomical and phenomenological factors. Therefore, it is likely that different processes (viz, perceptual vs motor; effortful vs habitual) of different aggression subtypes are a function of amygdala subregion and hemisphere.

In terms of functional abnormalities, pathological aggression is associated with a more labile range of amygdala activity, as well as differential amygdala responsivity to socially threatening stimuli. Commonly, aggression is associated with heightened amygdala reactivity to socially threatening stimuli (eg, fearful or angry faces), and, remarkably, responses to neutral interpersonal stimuli (eg, neutral facial expression) are also heightened. Amygdala hyporesponsiveness to threat, however, has also been observed, and this may be related to elements of aggression that are driven by the interpersonal/affective dimensions of psychopathy. Similar to the inconsistencies of amygdala volumetric changes, disentangling the role of amygdala hyper- vs hyporesponsiveness to interpersonally salient cues will also likely require resolving the amygdala into its component functional subregions and characterizing aggression subtypes and associated dimensions. Furthermore, the 5-HTergic system also appears to be involved in determining amygdala responses to neutral and aversive interpersonal stimuli, and this effect may also be subregion- and circuit-dependent.

An important issue that remains unexamined relates to the cellular and ultrastructural basis of amygdala volumetric alterations, as well as the mechanism by which such changes yield the above-described functional differences. Animal models suggest that morphological alterations in the amygdala may reflect decreased dendritic arborization of excitatoryReference Grillo, Risher and Macht 245 Reference Vyas, Bernal and Chattarji 248 and local inhibitory neurons,Reference Gilabert-Juan, Castillo-Gomez, Perez-Rando, Molto and Nacher 249 which could therefore disrupt both “top-down” cortical inhibition as well as local inhibitory processes. As stress hormones or glucocorticoids are involved in dendritic remodeling in limbic regions in animal models, a potential avenue for future studies may be determining whether the cortisol abnormalities observed in aggressive individuals are related to amygdala structural changes. Such a line of inquiry would also have the potential for developing therapeutic strategies, such as those that involve pharmacological manipulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis.

The OFC and ACC are limbic prefrontal regions that also play a key role in the neurobiology of aggression, particularly in terms of their interconnectivity with the amygdala. Involvement of the OFC and its coupling with the amygdala likely impairs (a) the ability to ascribe affective and motivational significance to stimuli in a manner that is integrated, moderated, and flexible; and (b) “top-down” modulation of central nucleus output, leading to an increased likelihood of a “visceral/sympathetic” component to a stimulus representation. Altered coupling between the ACC and amygdala suggests a disruption of effortful cognitive modulation of subcortical affect processing (pregenual ACC), as well as the development of negative self-referential emotional states (subgenual ACC).

A burgeoning literature has begun to illustrate the role of the striatum in aggression. Dysfunction of the ventral striatum likely contributes to aggression, owing to disturbances in the processing of expected outcome values, and therefore, phenomena such as “frustration.” Altered activity of the dorsomedial striatum may contribute to aggression by affecting the expected value or required effort associated with specific responses/actions.

Neurochemical systems are involved in aggression in at least 2 ways: (1) influencing central nervous system development during critical prenatal and postnatal periods and (2) modulating developed neural system/network parameters. The relationship between 5-HT availability during development and aggression is complex. For example, the absence of 5-HT synthesis (tph2 knock-out) as well as excessive 5-HT synthesis capacity (high-activity tph2 polymorphism) can yield an aggressive phenotype in rodent genetic models. Furthermore, the prenatal and postnatal effects of 5-HTT pharmacological inhibition in animal models also appear to differ, as they lead to increased and decreased aggression, respectively. The developmental role of DA is also beginning to be elucidated, and excessive DA availability may contribute to a particularly severe form of aggression. A number of gene x environment interactions have been described, including polymorphisms of tph2, 5-HTT, MAOA, the D4 receptor and COMT, and early adversity.

Low basal levels of presynaptic 5-HT, particularly in corticolimbic regions, such as the ACC, are believed to contribute to trait levels of aggression, possibly by diminishing the efficacy of cognitive control of amygdala and striatal processes. Increased 5-HT2A receptor function in the medial OFC may influence state levels of aggression, possibly by decreasing OFC-amygdala coupling. The 5-HT1B receptor appears poised to influence aggression by its presynaptic location on 5-HTergic and GABAergic neurons projecting from the raphe and striatum, respectively, to the midbrain DAergic system. The 5-HT3 receptor also may play a role in aggression through a number of potential mechanisms, such as presynaptic modulation of DA release in the striatum. While clearly implicated in mood and anxiety disorders, the contribution of the 5-HT1A receptor in aggression is less clear; however, it may play a role in impulsivity.

Greater DAergic synthesis and storage capacity may attenuate aggression, possibly by modulating error/reward signals in striatal regions and/or by enhancing cortical cognitive processes. The neuropeptide VIP may enhance aggression through its actions in the central nucleus of the amygdala, in a manner possibly antagonistic to that of 5-HT’s effect. The steroid hormones cortisol and testosterone interact to determine aggression and related constructs, such as psychopathy, possibly through their effects on amygdala–frontal connectivity. The standard view has been that low cortisol permits high testosterone to promote aggression; however, their inter-relationship appears to be more complex and dependent on a number of other variables.

There are a number of essential questions that deserve the focus of future studies. The first relates to the various forms of heterogeneity and potential confounds in the study of aggression: What are the convergent and divergent neural correlates of aggression subtypes, namely, reactive and proactive aggression? Further, how do the neural correlates of reactive and proactive aggression relate to those of their associated personality dimensions, viz, negative emotionality and impulsivity, and psychopathy, respectively? How does aggression differ between nonclinical participants, and personality disordered patients with and without IED? How do the various etiopathogenetic variants of aggression differ both phenomenologically and neurobiologically? And how do different etiopathogenetic factors interact—eg, MAOA and 5-HTT risk polymorphisms, and do these interactions possibly contribute to qualitative differences in aggression severity? In addition to these phenomenological and etiopathogenic factors, it will also be essential that neuroanatomical regions are well defined with respect to their functional subdivisions.

Future neurobiological studies will also offer the opportunity to address important clinical goals. In this review, we described various candidate therapeutic targets that deserve further characterization, such as the 5-HT1B and 5-HT3 receptors, the D4 receptor, and the AVP V1a and V1b receptors. Therefore, PET imaging studies of these receptors may help to clarify their role in the pathophysiology of aggression, and pharmacological challenges with ligands to these targets, in combination with brain imaging and laboratory aggression paradigms, may provide initial evidence as to their anti-aggressive efficacy.

The genetic and developmental studies described here have laid the groundwork for identifying biomarkers that could be used to identify at-risk individuals and to develop potential interventions to disrupt the pathogenesis of aggression. Clarifying, for example, the differential contribution of 5-HT vs DA in MAOA-dependent pathogenic mechanisms would allow for more rationally designed developmental interventions. Finally, as the neural circuitry of aggression is now better understood, dimensional biomarkers could be characterized that would represent sensitivity to specific treatments.

Disclosures

Daniel Rosell has the following disclosure: Dept. of Veterans Affairs, employee, salary. Larry Siever has the following disclosure: NIMH, researcher, salary/research support.

Footnotes

Writing of this manuscript was supported by Grant MH63875 from the National Institute of Mental Health to Larry J. Siever. Writing of this manuscript was also supported by The Department of Veterans Affairs Office of Academic Affiliations Psychiatric Research/Neurosciences Advanced Fellowship.

References

1. Hall, JE, Simon, TR, Lee, RD, Mercy, JA. Implications of direct protective factors for public health research and prevention strategies to reduce youth violence. Am J Prev Med. 2012; 43(2 Suppl 1): S76S83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2. Modi, MN, Palmer, S, Armstrong, A. The role of Violence Against Women Act in addressing intimate partner violence: a public health issue. J Womens Health (Larchmt). 2014; 23(3): 253259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Ramsay, SE, Bartley, A, Rodger, AJ. Determinants of assault-related violence in the community: potential for public health interventions in hospitals. Emerg Med J. 2014; 31: 986989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4. Whitaker, S. Preventing violent conflict: a revised mandate for the public health professional? J Public Health Policy. 2013; 34(1): 4654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Bushman, BJ, Anderson, CA. Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychol Rev. 2001; 108(1): 273279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Baker, LA, Raine, A, Liu, J, Jacobson, KC. Differential genetic and environmental influences on reactive and proactive aggression in children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008; 36(8): 12651278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. Bezdjian, S, Tuvblad, C, Raine, A, Baker, LA. The genetic and environmental covariation among psychopathic personality traits, and reactive and proactive aggression in childhood. Child Dev. 2011; 82(4): 12671281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Cima, M, Raine, A, Meesters, C, Popma, A. Validation of the Dutch Reactive Proactive Questionnaire (RPQ): differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression from childhood to adulthood. Aggress Behav. 2013; 39(2): 99113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Fossati, A, Raine, A, Borroni, S, et al. A cross-cultural study of the psychometric properties of the Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire among Italian nonclinical adolescents. Psychol Assess. 2009; 21(1): 131135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Fung, AL, Raine, A, Gao, Y. Cross-cultural generalizability of the Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ). J Pers Assess. 2009; 91(5): 473479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Raine, A, Dodge, K, Loeber, R, et al. The Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggress Behav. 2006; 32(2): 159171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Gardner, KJ, Archer, J, Jackson, S. Does maladaptive coping mediate the relationship between borderline personality traits and reactive and proactive aggression? Aggress Behav. 2012; 38(5): 403413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Lobbestael, J, Cima, M, Arntz, A. The relationship between adult reactive and proactive aggression, hostile interpretation bias, and antisocial personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 2013; 27(1): 5366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Dodge, KA, Lochman, JE, Harnish, JD, Bates, JE, Pettit, GS. Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997; 106(1): 3751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. Kolla, NJ, Malcolm, C, Attard, S, Arenovich, T, Blackwood, N, Hodgins, S. Childhood maltreatment and aggressive behaviour in violent offenders with psychopathy. Can J Psychiatry. 2013; 58(8): 487494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Arsenio, WF, Adams, E, Gold, J. Social information processing, moral reasoning, and emotion attributions: relations with adolescents’ reactive and proactive aggression. Child Dev. 2009; 80(6): 17391755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Crick, NR, Dodge, KA. Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Dev. 1996; 67(3): 9931002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18. Dodge, KA, Coie, JD. Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987; 53(6): 11461158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Hubbard, JA, Dodge, KA, Cillessen, AH, Coie, JD, Schwartz, D. The dyadic nature of social information processing in boys’ reactive and proactive aggression. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001; 80(2): 268280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. Smithmyer, CM, Hubbard, JA, Simons, RF. Proactive and reactive aggression in delinquent adolescents: relations to aggression outcome expectancies. J Clin Child Psychol. 2000; 29(1): 8693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21. Walters, GD. Measuring proactive and reactive criminal thinking with the PICTS: correlations with outcome expectancies and hostile attribution biases. J Interpers Violence. 2007; 22(4): 371385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Brugman, S, Lobbestael, J, Arntz, A, et al. Identifying cognitive predictors of reactive and proactive aggression. Aggress Behav. In press. DOI: 10.1002/AB.21573.Google Scholar
23. Coccaro, EF, Kavoussi, RJ, Berman, ME, Lish, JD. Intermittent explosive disorder—revised: development, reliability, and validity of research criteria. Compr Psychiatry. 1998; 39(6): 368376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. McCloskey, MS, Berman, ME, Noblett, KL, Coccaro, EF. Intermittent explosive disorder-integrated research diagnostic criteria: convergent and discriminant validity. J Psychiatr Res. 2006; 40(3): 231242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Coccaro, EF. Intermittent explosive disorder: development of integrated research criteria for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. Compr Psychiatry. 2011; 52(2): 119125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26. Coccaro, EF, Lee, R, Kavoussi, RJ. Aggression, suicidality, and intermittent explosive disorder: serotonergic correlates in personality disorder and healthy control subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35(2): 435444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Salzman, CD, Fusi, S. Emotion, cognition, and mental state representation in amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2010; 33: 173202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. Fernando, AB, Murray, JE, Milton, AL. The amygdala: securing pleasure and avoiding pain. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013; 7: 190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29. Sah, P, Faber, ES, Lopez De, AM, Power, J. The amygdaloid complex: anatomy and physiology. Physiol Rev. 2003; 83(3): 803834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30. John, YJ, Bullock, D, Zikopoulos, B, Barbas, H. Anatomy and computational modeling of networks underlying cognitive-emotional interaction. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013; 7: 101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31. Lee, S, Kim, SJ, Kwon, OB, Lee, JH, Kim, JH. Inhibitory networks of the amygdala for emotional memory. Front Neural Circuits. 2013; 7: 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32. Bzdok, D, Laird, AR, Zilles, K, Fox, PT, Eickhoff, SB. An investigation of the structural, connectional, and functional subspecialization in the human amygdala. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013; 34(12): 32473266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Matthies, S, Rusch, N, Weber, M, et al. Small amygdala-high aggression? The role of the amygdala in modulating aggression in healthy subjects. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2012; 13(1): 7581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Pardini, DA, Raine, A, Erickson, K, Loeber, R. Lower amygdala volume in men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic traits, and future violence. Biol Psychiatry. 2014; 75(1): 7380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35. Gopal, A, Clark, E, Allgair, A, et al. Dorsal/ventral parcellation of the amygdala: relevance to impulsivity and aggression. Psychiatry Res. 2013; 211(1): 2430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Bobes, MA, Ostrosky, F, Diaz, K, et al. Linkage of functional and structural anomalies in the left amygdala of reactive-aggressive men. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013; 8(8): 928936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. Dyck, M, Loughead, J, Kellermann, T, Boers, F, Gur, RC, Mathiak, K. Cognitive versus automatic mechanisms of mood induction differentially activate left and right amygdala. Neuroimage. 2011; 54(3): 25032513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38. New, AS, Hazlett, EA, Newmark, RE, et al. Laboratory induced aggression: a positron emission tomography study of aggressive individuals with borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009; 66(12): 11071114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39. Coccaro, EF, McCloskey, MS, Fitzgerald, DA, Phan, KL. Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007; 62(2): 168178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. Lozier, LM, Cardinale, EM, Vanmeter, JW, Marsh, AA. Mediation of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression by amygdala response to fear among children with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014; 71(6): 627636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41. Walton, ME, Croxson, PL, Behrens, TE, Kennerley, SW, Rushworth, MF. Adaptive decision making and value in the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage. 2007; 36(Suppl 2): T142T154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42. Rudebeck, PH, Murray, EA. The orbitofrontal oracle: cortical mechanisms for the prediction and evaluation of specific behavioral outcomes. Neuron. 2014; 84(6): 11431156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43. Gansler, DA, McLaughlin, NC, Iguchi, L, et al. A multivariate approach to aggression and the orbital frontal cortex in psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Res. 2009; 171(3): 145154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44. Antonucci, AS, Gansler, DA, Tan, S, Bhadelia, R, Patz, S, Fulwiler, C. Orbitofrontal correlates of aggression and impulsivity in psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Res. 2006; 147(2–3): 213220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45. Boes, AD, Tranel, D, Anderson, SW, Nopoulos, P. Right anterior cingulate: a neuroanatomical correlate of aggression and defiance in boys. Behav Neurosci. 2008; 122(3): 677684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46. Ducharme, S, Hudziak, JJ, Botteron, KN, et al. Right anterior cingulate cortical thickness and bilateral striatal volume correlate with child behavior checklist aggressive behavior scores in healthy children. Biol Psychiatry. 2011; 70(3): 283290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47. Soloff, PH, Meltzer, CC, Greer, PJ, Constantine, D, Kelly, TM. A fenfluramine-activated FDG-PET study of borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2000; 47(6): 540547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48. New, AS, Hazlett, EA, Buchsbaum, MS, et al. Blunted prefrontal cortical 18fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography response to meta-chlorophenylpiperazine in impulsive aggression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002; 59(7): 621629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49. Ghashghaei, HT, Hilgetag, CC, Barbas, H. Sequence of information processing for emotions based on the anatomic dialogue between prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Neuroimage. 2007; 34(3): 905923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50. Timbie, C, Barbas, H. Specialized pathways from the primate amygdala to posterior orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2014; 34(24): 81068118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51. Ghashghaei, HT, Barbas, H. Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience. 2002; 115(4): 12611279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52. Roy, AK, Shehzad, Z, Margulies, DS, et al. Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2009; 45(2): 614626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53. Hoptman, MJ, D’Angelo, D, Catalano, D, et al. Amygdalofrontal functional disconnectivity and aggression in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2010; 36(5): 10201028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54. Fulwiler, CE, King, JA, Zhang, N. Amygdala-orbitofrontal resting-state functional connectivity is associated with trait anger. Neuroreport. 2012; 23(10): 606610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55. Beyer, F, Munte, TF, Wiechert, J, Heldmann, M, Kramer, UM. Trait aggressiveness is not related to structural connectivity between orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala. PLoS One. 2014; 9(6): e101105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
56. New, AS, Hazlett, EA, Buchsbaum, MS, et al. Amygdala-prefrontal disconnection in borderline personality disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007; 32(7): 16291640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57. Hornboll, B, Macoveanu, J, Rowe, J, et al. Acute serotonin 2A receptor blocking alters the processing of fearful faces in the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala. J Psychopharmacol. 2013; 27(10): 903914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58. Rosell, DR, Thompson, JL, Slifstein, M, et al. Increased serotonin 2A receptor availability in the orbitofrontal cortex of physically aggressive personality disordered patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 67(12): 11541162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59. Passamonti, L, Crockett, MJ, Apergis-Schoute, AM, et al. Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on prefrontal-amygdala connectivity while viewing facial signals of aggression. Biol Psychiatry. 2012; 71(1): 3643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60. Pezawas, L, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Drabant, EM, et al. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8(6): 828834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
61. Heinz, A, Braus, DF, Smolka, MN, et al. Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic variation of the serotonin transporter. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8(1): 2021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62. Haber, SN. The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks. J Chem Neuroanat. 2003; 26(4): 317330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63. Crockett, MJ, Apergis-Schoute, A, Herrmann, B, et al. Serotonin modulates striatal responses to fairness and retaliation in humans. J Neurosci. 2013; 33(8): 35053513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64. van de Giessen, E, Rosell, DR, Thompson, JL, et al. Serotonin transporter availability in impulsive aggressive personality disordered patients: a PET study with [(11)C]DASB. J Psychiatr Res. 2014; 58: 147154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
65. Malick, JB, Barnett, A. The role of serotonergic pathways in isolation-induced aggression in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1976; 5(1): 5561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66. Brown, GL, Goodwin, FK, Ballenger, JC, Goyer, PF, Major, LF. Aggression in humans correlates with cerebrospinal fluid amine metabolites. Psychiatry Res. 1979; 1(2): 131139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
67. Brown, GL, Ebert, MH, Goyer, PF, et al. Aggression, suicide, and serotonin: relationships to CSF amine metabolites. Am J Psychiatry. 1982; 139(6): 741746.Google ScholarPubMed
68. Brown, CS, Kent, TA, Bryant, SG, et al. Blood platelet uptake of serotonin in episodic aggression. Psychiatry Res. 1989; 27(1): 512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69. Stoff, DM, Pollock, L, Vitiello, B, Behar, D, Bridger, WH. Reduction of (3H)-imipramine binding sites on platelets of conduct-disordered children. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1987; 1(1): 5562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70. Coccaro, EF, Kavoussi, RJ, Hauger, RL. Physiological responses to d-fenfluramine and ipsapirone challenge correlate with indices of aggression in males with personality disorder. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 1995; 10(3): 177179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71. Coccaro, EF, Berman, ME, Kavoussi, RJ, Hauger, RL. Relationship of prolactin response to d-fenfluramine to behavioral and questionnaire assessments of aggression in personality-disordered men. Biol Psychiatry. 1996; 40(3): 157164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
72. Coccaro, EF, Kavoussi, RJ, Cooper, TB, Hauger, RL. Central serotonin activity and aggression: inverse relationship with prolactin response to d-fenfluramine, but not CSF 5-HIAA concentration, in human subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 1997; 154(10): 14301435.Google Scholar
73. Coccaro, EF, Astill, JL, Herbert, JL, Schut, AG. Fluoxetine treatment of impulsive aggression in DSM-III-R personality disorder patients. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1990; 10(5): 373375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
74. Coccaro, EF, Kavoussi, RJ. Fluoxetine and impulsive aggressive behavior in personality-disordered subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997; 54(12): 10811088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
75. Kruesi, MJ, Rapoport, JL, Hamburger, S, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid monoamine metabolites, aggression, and impulsivity in disruptive behavior disorders of children and adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990; 47(5): 419426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
76. Coccaro, EF, Lee, R. Cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindolacetic acid and homovanillic acid: reciprocal relationships with impulsive aggression in human subjects. J Neural Transm. 2010; 117(2): 241248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
77. Coccaro, EF, Kavoussi, RJ, Hauger, RL, Cooper, TB, Ferris, CF. Cerebrospinal fluid vasopressin levels: correlates with aggression and serotonin function in personality-disordered subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998; 55(8): 708714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
78. Goveas, JS, Csernansky, JG, Coccaro, EF. Platelet serotonin content correlates inversely with life history of aggression in personality-disordered subjects. Psychiatry Res. 2004; 126(1): 2332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
79. Coccaro, EF, Kavoussi, RJ, Sheline, YI, Berman, ME, Csernansky, JG. Impulsive aggression in personality disorder correlates with platelet 5-HT2A receptor binding. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997; 16(3): 211216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
80. Marseille, R, Lee, R, Coccaro, EF. Inter-relationship between different platelet measures of 5-HT and their relationship to aggression in human subjects. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2012; 36(2): 277281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
81. Modai, I, Gibel, A, Rauchverger, B, Ritsner, M, Klein, E, Ben-Shachar, D. Paroxetine binding in aggressive schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Res. 2000; 94(1): 7781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
82. Sarne, Y, Mandel, J, Goncalves, MH, Brook, S, Gafni, M, Elizur, A. Imipramine binding to blood platelets and aggressive behavior in offenders, schizophrenics and normal volunteers. Neuropsychobiology. 1995; 31(3): 120124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
83. Coccaro, EF, Kavoussi, RJ, Hauger, RL. Serotonin function and antiaggressive response to fluoxetine: a pilot study. Biol Psychiatry. 1997; 42(7): 546552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
84. Carpenter, LL, Anderson, GM, Pelton, GH, et al. Tryptophan depletion during continuous CSF sampling in healthy human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998; 19(1): 2635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
85. Moreno, FA, McGavin, C, Malan, TP, et al. Tryptophan depletion selectively reduces CSF 5-HT metabolites in healthy young men: results from single lumbar puncture sampling technique. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2000; 3(4): 277283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
86. Williams, WA, Shoaf, SE, Hommer, D, Rawlings, R, Linnoila, M. Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on plasma and cerebrospinal fluid tryptophan and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in normal volunteers. J Neurochem. 1999; 72(4): 16411647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
87. Bjork, JM, Dougherty, DM, Moeller, FG, Cherek, DR, Swann, AC. The effects of tryptophan depletion and loading on laboratory aggression in men: time course and a food-restricted control. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999; 142(1): 2430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
88. Bjork, JM, Dougherty, DM, Moeller, FG, Swann, AC. Differential behavioral effects of plasma tryptophan depletion and loading in aggressive and nonaggressive men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000; 22(4): 357369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
89. Kramer, UM, Riba, J, Richter, S, Munte, TF. An fMRI study on the role of serotonin in reactive aggression. PLoS One. 2011; 6(11): e27668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
90. Kotting, WF, Bubenzer, S, Helmbold, K, Eisert, A, Gaber, TJ, Zepf, FD. Effects of tryptophan depletion on reactive aggression and aggressive decision-making in young people with ADHD. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013; 128(2): 114123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
91. Stadler, C, Zepf, FD, Demisch, L, Schmitt, M, Landgraf, M, Poustka, F. Influence of rapid tryptophan depletion on laboratory-provoked aggression in children with ADHD. Neuropsychobiology. 2007; 56(2–3): 104110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
92. Zimmermann, M, Grabemann, M, Mette, C, et al. The effects of acute tryptophan depletion on reactive aggression in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and healthy controls. PLoS One. 2012; 7(3): e32023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
93. Fanning, JR, Berman, ME, Guillot, CR, Marsic, A, McCloskey, MS. Serotonin (5-HT) augmentation reduces provoked aggression associated with primary psychopathy traits. J Pers Disord. 2014; 28(3): 449461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
94. Berman, ME, McCloskey, MS, Fanning, JR, Schumacher, JA, Coccaro, EF. Serotonin augmentation reduces response to attack in aggressive individuals. Psychol Sci. 2009; 20(6): 714720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
95. Rubia, K, Lee, F, Cleare, AJ, et al. Tryptophan depletion reduces right inferior prefrontal activation during response inhibition in fast, event-related fMRI. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2005; 179(4): 791803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
96. Lee, RJ, Gill, A, Chen, B, McCloskey, M, Coccaro, EF. Modulation of central serotonin affects emotional information processing in impulsive aggressive personality disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2012; 32(3): 329335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
97. Grady, CL, Siebner, HR, Hornboll, B, Macoveanu, J, Paulson, OB, Knudsen, GM. Acute pharmacologically induced shifts in serotonin availability abolish emotion-selective responses to negative face emotions in distinct brain networks. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013; 23(5): 368378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
98. Zhang, X, Beaulieu, JM, Sotnikova, TD, Gainetdinov, RR, Caron, MG. Tryptophan hydroxylase-2 controls brain serotonin synthesis. Science. 2004; 305(5681): 217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
99. Osipova, DV, Kulikov, AV, Popova, NK. C1473G polymorphism in mouse tph2 gene is linked to tryptophan hydroxylase-2 activity in the brain, intermale aggression, and depressive-like behavior in the forced swim test. J Neurosci Res. 2009; 87(5): 11681174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
100. Takahashi, A, Shiroishi, T, Koide, T. Genetic mapping of escalated aggression in wild-derived mouse strain MSM/Ms: association with serotonin-related genes. Front Neurosci. 2014; 8: 156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
101. Mosienko, V, Bert, B, Beis, D, et al. Exaggerated aggression and decreased anxiety in mice deficient in brain serotonin. Transl Psychiatry. 2012; 2: e122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
102. Chen, GL, Novak, MA, Meyer, JS, Kelly, BJ, Vallender, EJ, Miller, GM. The effect of rearing experience and TPH2 genotype on HPA axis function and aggression in rhesus monkeys: a retrospective analysis. Horm Behav. 2010; 57(2): 184191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
103. Yang, J, Lee, MS, Lee, SH, et al. Association between tryptophan hydroxylase 2 polymorphism and anger-related personality traits among young Korean women. Neuropsychobiology. 2010; 62(3): 158163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
104. Yoon, HK, Lee, HJ, Kim, L, Lee, MS, Ham, BJ. Impact of tryptophan hydroxylase 2 G-703T polymorphism on anger-related personality traits and orbitofrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res. 2012; 231(1): 105110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
105. Gutknecht, L, Jacob, C, Strobel, A, et al. Tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variation influences personality traits and disorders related to emotional dysregulation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007; 10(3): 309320.Google ScholarPubMed
106. Inoue, H, Yamasue, H, Tochigi, M, et al. Effect of tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variants on amygdalar and hippocampal volumes. Brain Res. 2010; 1331: 5157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
107. Perez-Rodriguez, MM, Weinstein, S, New, AS, et al. Tryptophan-hydroxylase 2 haplotype association with borderline personality disorder and aggression in a sample of patients with personality disorders and healthy controls. J Psychiatr Res. 2010; 44(15): 10751081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
108. Brown, SM, Peet, E, Manuck, SB, et al. A regulatory variant of the human tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene biases amygdala reactivity. Mol Psychiatry. 2005; 10(9): 884888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
109. Booij, L, Turecki, G, Leyton, M, et al. Tryptophan hydroxylase(2) gene polymorphisms predict brain serotonin synthesis in the orbitofrontal cortex in humans. Mol Psychiatry. 2012; 17(8): 809817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
110. Heiming, RS, Monning, A, Jansen, F, Kloke, V, Lesch, KP, Sachser, N. To attack, or not to attack? The role of serotonin transporter genotype in the display of maternal aggression. Behav Brain Res. 2013; 242: 135141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
111. Holmes, A, Murphy, DL, Crawley, JN. Reduced aggression in mice lacking the serotonin transporter. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2002; 161(2): 160167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
112. Yu, Q, Teixeira, CM, Mahadevia, D, et al. Dopamine and serotonin signaling during two sensitive developmental periods differentially impact adult aggressive and affective behaviors in mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2014; 19(6): 688698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
113. Kiryanova, V, Dyck, RH. Increased aggression, improved spatial memory, and reduced anxiety-like behaviour in adult male mice exposed to fluoxetine early in life. Dev Neurosci. 2014; 36(5): 396408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
114. Heils, A, Teufel, A, Petri, S, et al. Allelic variation of human serotonin transporter gene expression. J Neurochem. 1996; 66(6): 26212624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
115. May, ME, Lightfoot, DA, Srour, A, Kowalchuk, RK, Kennedy, CH. Association between serotonin transporter polymorphisms and problem behavior in adult males with intellectual disabilities. Brain Res. 2010; 1357: 97103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
116. Hallikainen, T, Saito, T, Lachman, HM, et al. Association between low activity serotonin transporter promoter genotype and early onset alcoholism with habitual impulsive violent behavior. Mol Psychiatry. 1999; 4(4): 385388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
117. Haberstick, BC, Smolen, A, Hewitt, JK. Family-based association test of the 5HTTLPR and aggressive behavior in a general population sample of children. Biol Psychiatry. 2006; 59(9): 836843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
118. Beitchman, JH, Baldassarra, L, Mik, H, et al. Serotonin transporter polymorphisms and persistent, pervasive childhood aggression. Am J Psychiatry. 2006; 163(6): 11031105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
119. Verona, E, Joiner, TE, Johnson, F, Bender, TW. Gender specific gene-environment interactions on laboratory-assessed aggression. Biol Psychol. 2006; 71(1): 3341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
120. Conway, CC, Keenan-Miller, D, Hammen, C, Lind, PA, Najman, JM, Brennan, PA. Coaction of stress and serotonin transporter genotype in predicting aggression at the transition to adulthood. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2012; 41(1): 5363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
121. Reif, A, Rosler, M, Freitag, CM, et al. Nature and nurture predispose to violent behavior: serotonergic genes and adverse childhood environment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007; 32(11): 23752383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
122. Aluja, A, Garcia, LF, Blanch, A, De, LD, Fibla, J. Impulsive-disinhibited personality and serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms: association study in an inmate’s sample. J Psychiatr Res. 2009; 43(10): 906914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
123. Payer, DE, Nurmi, EL, Wilson, SA, McCracken, JT, London, ED. Effects of methamphetamine abuse and serotonin transporter gene variants on aggression and emotion-processing neurocircuitry. Transl Psychiatry. 2012; 2: e80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
124. Philibert, R, Madan, A, Andersen, A, Cadoret, R, Packer, H, Sandhu, H. Serotonin transporter mRNA levels are associated with the methylation of an upstream CpG island. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007; 144B(1): 101105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
125. Koenen, KC, Uddin, M, Chang, SC, et al. SLC6A4 methylation modifies the effect of the number of traumatic events on risk for posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2011; 28(8): 639647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
126. Zhao, J, Goldberg, J, Bremner, JD, Vaccarino, V. Association between promoter methylation of serotonin transporter gene and depressive symptoms: a monozygotic twin study. Psychosom Med. 2013; 75(6): 523529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
127. Dannlowski, U, Kugel, H, Redlich, R, et al. Serotonin transporter gene methylation is associated with hippocampal gray matter volume. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014; 35(11): 53565367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
128. Nikolova, YS, Koenen, KC, Galea, S, et al. Beyond genotype: serotonin transporter epigenetic modification predicts human brain function. Nat Neurosci. 2014; 17(9): 11531155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
129. Wang, D, Szyf, M, Benkelfat, C, et al. Peripheral SLC6A4 DNA methylation is associated with in vivo measures of human brain serotonin synthesis and childhood physical aggression. PLoS One. 2012; 7(6): e39501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
130. Frankle, WG, Lombardo, I, New, AS, et al. Brain serotonin transporter distribution in subjects with impulsive aggressivity: a positron emission study with [11C]McN 5652. Am J Psychiatry. 2005; 162(5): 915923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
131. Bortolato, M, Chen, K, Shih, JC. Monoamine oxidase inactivation: from pathophysiology to therapeutics. Adv Drug Deliv Rev. 2008; 60(13–14): 15271533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
132. Brunner, HG, Nelen, M, Breakefield, XO, Ropers, HH, van Oost, BA. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science. 1993; 262(5133): 578580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
133. Sabol, SZ, Hu, S, Hamer, D. A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Hum Genet. 1998; 103(3): 273279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
134. Kuepper, Y, Grant, P, Wielpuetz, C, Hennig, J. MAOA-uVNTR genotype predicts interindividual differences in experimental aggressiveness as a function of the degree of provocation. Behav Brain Res. 2013; 247: 7378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
135. Manuck, SB, Flory, JD, Ferrell, RE, Mann, JJ, Muldoon, MF. A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry Res. 2000; 95(1): 923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
136. Stetler, DA, Davis, C, Leavitt, K, et al. Association of low-activity MAOA allelic variants with violent crime in incarcerated offenders. J Psychiatr Res. 2014; 58: 6975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
137. Byrd, AL, Manuck, SB. MAOA, childhood maltreatment, and antisocial behavior: meta-analysis of a gene-environment interaction. Biol Psychiatry. 2014; 75(1): 917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
138. Karere, GM, Kinnally, EL, Sanchez, JN, Famula, TR, Lyons, LA, Capitanio, JP. What is an “adverse” environment? Interactions of rearing experiences and MAOA genotype in rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry. 2009; 65(9): 770777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
139. Newman, TK, Syagailo, YV, Barr, CS, et al. Monoamine oxidase A gene promoter variation and rearing experience influences aggressive behavior in rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry. 2005; 57(2): 167172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
140. Alia-Klein, N, Goldstein, RZ, Kriplani, A, et al. Brain monoamine oxidase A activity predicts trait aggression. J Neurosci. 2008; 28(19): 50995104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
141. Cases, O, Seif, I, Grimsby, J, et al. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science. 1995; 268(5218): 17631766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
142. Scott, AL, Bortolato, M, Chen, K, Shih, JC. Novel monoamine oxidase A knock out mice with human-like spontaneous mutation. Neuroreport. 2008; 19(7): 739743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
143. Cases, O, Vitalis, T, Seif, I, De Maeyer, E, Sotelo, C, Gaspar, P. Lack of barrels in the somatosensory cortex of monoamine oxidase A-deficient mice: role of a serotonin excess during the critical period. Neuron. 1996; 16(2): 297307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
144. Bortolato, M, Godar, SC, Tambaro, S, et al. Early postnatal inhibition of serotonin synthesis results in long-term reductions of perseverative behaviors, but not aggression, in MAO A-deficient mice. Neuropharmacology. 2013; 75: 223232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
145. Garcia-Garcia, AL, Newman-Tancredi, A, Leonardo, ED. 5-HT(1A) [corrected] receptors in mood and anxiety: recent insights into autoreceptor versus heteroreceptor function. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2014; 231(4): 623636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
146. Polter, AM, Li, X. 5-HT1A receptor-regulated signal transduction pathways in brain. Cell Signal. 2010; 22(10): 14061412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
147. Miczek, KA, Hussain, S, Faccidomo, S. Alcohol-heightened aggression in mice: attenuation by 5-HT1A receptor agonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1998; 139(1–2): 160168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
148. Pruus, K, Skrebuhhova-Malmros, T, Rudissaar, R, Matto, V, Allikmets, L. 5-HT1A receptor agonists buspirone and gepirone attenuate apomorphine-induced aggressive behaviour in adult male Wistar rats. J Physiol Pharmacol. 2000; 51(4 Pt 2): 833846.Google ScholarPubMed
149. Centenaro, LA, Vieira, K, Zimmermann, N, Miczek, KA, Lucion, AB, de Almeida, RM. Social instigation and aggressive behavior in mice: role of 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors in the prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008; 201(2): 237248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
150. da Veiga, CP, Miczek, KA, Lucion, AB, de Almeida, RM. Social instigation and aggression in postpartum female rats: role of 5-Ht1A and 5-Ht1B receptors in the dorsal raphe nucleus and prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011; 213(2–3): 475487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
151. de Boer, SF, Lesourd, M, Mocaer, E, Koolhaas, JM. Somatodendritic 5-HT(1A) autoreceptors mediate the anti-aggressive actions of 5-HT(1A) receptor agonists in rats: an ethopharmacological study with S-15535, alnespirone, and WAY-100635. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000; 23(1): 2033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
152. Stein, DJ, Miczek, KA, Lucion, AB, de Almeida, RM. Aggression-reducing effects of F15599, a novel selective 5-HT1A receptor agonist, after microinjection into the ventral orbital prefrontal cortex, but not in infralimbic cortex in male mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013; 230(3): 375387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
153. Naumenko, VS, Kozhemyakina, RV, Plyusnina, IF, Kulikov, AV, Popova, NK. Serotonin 5-HT1A receptor in infancy-onset aggression: comparison with genetically defined aggression in adult rats. Behav Brain Res. 2013; 243: 97101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
154. Popova, NK, Naumenko, VS, Plyusnina, IZ, Kulikov, AV. Reduction in 5-HT1A receptor density, 5-HT1A mRNA expression, and functional correlates for 5-HT1A receptors in genetically defined aggressive rats. J Neurosci Res. 2005; 80(2): 286292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
155. Popova, NK, Naumenko, VS, Plyusnina, IZ. Involvement of brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptors in genetic predisposition to aggressive behavior. Neurosci Behav Physiol. 2007; 37(6): 631635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
156. van der Vegt, BJ, de Boer, SF, Buwalda, B, de Ruiter, AJ, de Jong, JG, Koolhaas, JM. Enhanced sensitivity of postsynaptic serotonin-1A receptors in rats and mice with high trait aggression. Physiol Behav. 2001; 74(1–2): 205211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
157. Audero, E, Mlinar, B, Baccini, G, Skachokova, ZK, Corradetti, R, Gross, C. Suppression of serotonin neuron firing increases aggression in mice. J Neurosci. 2013; 33(20): 86788688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
158. Almeida, M, Lee, R, Coccaro, EF. Cortisol responses to ipsapirone challenge correlate with aggression, while basal cortisol levels correlate with impulsivity, in personality disorder and healthy volunteer subjects. J Psychiatr Res. 2010; 44(14): 874880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
159. Coccaro, EF, Gabriel, S, Siever, LJ. Buspirone challenge: preliminary evidence for a role for central 5-HT1a receptor function in impulsive aggressive behavior in humans. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1990; 26(3): 393405.Google ScholarPubMed
160. Benko, A, Lazary, J, Molnar, E, et al. Significant association between the C(-1019)G functional polymorphism of the HTR1A gene and impulsivity. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2010; 153B(2): 592599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
161. Joyce, PR, Stephenson, J, Kennedy, M, Mulder, RT, McHugh, PC. The presence of both serotonin 1A receptor (HTR1A) and dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene variants increase the risk of borderline personality disorder. Front Genet. 2014; 4: 313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
162. Witte, AV, Floel, A, Stein, P, et al. Aggression is related to frontal serotonin-1A receptor distribution as revealed by PET in healthy subjects. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009; 30(8): 25582570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
163. Parsey, RV, Oquendo, MA, Simpson, NR, et al. Effects of sex, age, and aggressive traits in man on brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding potential measured by PET using [C-11]WAY-100635. Brain Res. 2002; 954(2): 173182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
164. Sari, Y. Serotonin1B receptors: from protein to physiological function and behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004; 28(6): 565582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
165. Bannai, M, Fish, EW, Faccidomo, S, Miczek, KA. Anti-aggressive effects of agonists at 5-HT1B receptors in the dorsal raphe nucleus of mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2007; 193(2): 295304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
166. de Almeida, RM, Miczek, KA. Aggression escalated by social instigation or by discontinuation of reinforcement (“frustration”) in mice: inhibition by anpirtoline: a 5-HT1B receptor agonist. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002; 27(2): 171181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
167. de Almeida, RM, Rosa, MM, Santos, DM, Saft, DM, Benini, Q, Miczek, KA. 5-HT(1B) receptors, ventral orbitofrontal cortex, and aggressive behavior in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2006; 185(4): 441450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
168. Faccidomo, S, Bannai, M, Miczek, KA. Escalated aggression after alcohol drinking in male mice: dorsal raphe and prefrontal cortex serotonin and 5-HT(1B) receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008; 33(12): 28882899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
169. Faccidomo, S, Quadros, IM, Takahashi, A, Fish, EW, Miczek, KA. Infralimbic and dorsal raphe microinjection of the 5-HT(1B) receptor agonist CP-93,129: attenuation of aggressive behavior in CFW male mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012; 222(1): 117128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
170. Fish, EW, Faccidomo, S, Miczek, KA. Aggression heightened by alcohol or social instigation in mice: reduction by the 5-HT(1B) receptor agonist CP-94,253. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999; 146(4): 391399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
171. Fish, EW, McKenzie-Quirk, SD, Bannai, M, Miczek, KA. 5-HT(1B) receptor inhibition of alcohol-heightened aggression in mice: comparison to drinking and running. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008; 197(1): 145156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
172. Veiga, CP, Miczek, KA, Lucion, AB, Almeida, RM. Effect of 5-HT1B receptor agonists injected into the prefrontal cortex on maternal aggression in rats. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2007; 40(6): 825830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
173. Ramboz, S, Saudou, F, Amara, DA, et al. 5-HT1B receptor knock out—behavioral consequences. Behav Brain Res. 1996; 73(1–2): 305312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
174. Bouwknecht, JA, Hijzen, TH, van der Gugten, J, Maes, RA, Hen, R, Olivier, B. Absence of 5-HT(1B) receptors is associated with impaired impulse control in male 5-HT(1B) knockout mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2001; 49(7): 557568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
175. Conner, TS, Jensen, KP, Tennen, H, Furneaux, HM, Kranzler, HR, Covault, J. Functional polymorphisms in the serotonin 1B receptor gene (HTR1B) predict self-reported anger and hostility among young men. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2010; 153B(1): 6778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
176. Hakulinen, C, Jokela, M, Hintsanen, M, et al. Serotonin receptor 1B genotype and hostility, anger and aggressive behavior through the lifespan: the Young Finns study. J Behav Med. 2013; 36(6): 583590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
177. Jensen, KP, Covault, J, Conner, TS, Tennen, H, Kranzler, HR, Furneaux, HM. A common polymorphism in serotonin receptor 1B mRNA moderates regulation by miR-96 and associates with aggressive human behaviors. Mol Psychiatry. 2009; 14(4): 381389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
178. Zouk, H, McGirr, A, Lebel, V, Benkelfat, C, Rouleau, G, Turecki, G. The effect of genetic variation of the serotonin 1B receptor gene on impulsive aggressive behavior and suicide. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007; 144B(8): 9961002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
179. Potenza, MN, Walderhaug, E, Henry, S, et al. Serotonin 1B receptor imaging in pathological gambling. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2013; 14(2): 139145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
180. Hu, J, Henry, S, Gallezot, JD, et al. Serotonin 1B receptor imaging in alcohol dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 67(9): 800803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
181. Murrough, JW, Henry, S, Hu, J, et al. Reduced ventral striatal/ventral pallidal serotonin1B receptor binding potential in major depressive disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011; 213(2–3): 547553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
182. Higgins, GA, Enderlin, M, Haman, M, Fletcher, PJ. The 5-HT2A receptor antagonist M100,907 attenuates motor and ‘impulsive-type’ behaviours produced by NMDA receptor antagonism. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2003; 170(3): 309319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
183. Sakaue, M, Ago, Y, Sowa, C, et al. Modulation by 5-hT2A receptors of aggressive behavior in isolated mice. Jpn J Pharmacol. 2002; 89(1): 8992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
184. Winstanley, CA, Theobald, DE, Dalley, JW, Glennon, JC, Robbins, TW. 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor antagonists have opposing effects on a measure of impulsivity: interactions with global 5-HT depletion. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004; 176(3–4): 376385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
185. Giegling, I, Hartmann, AM, Moller, HJ, Rujescu, D. Anger- and aggression-related traits are associated with polymorphisms in the 5-HT-2A gene. J Affect Disord. 2006; 96(1–2): 7581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
186. Bruce, KR, Steiger, H, Joober, R, Ng Ying Kin, NM, Israel, M, Young, SN. Association of the promoter polymorphism -1438G/A of the 5-HT2A receptor gene with behavioral impulsiveness and serotonin function in women with bulimia nervosa. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005; 137B(1): 4044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
187. Jakubczyk, A, Wrzosek, M, Lukaszkiewicz, J, et al. The CC genotype in HTR2A T102C polymorphism is associated with behavioral impulsivity in alcohol-dependent patients. J Psychiatr Res. 2012; 46(1): 4449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
188. Jakubczyk, A, Klimkiewicz, A, Kopera, M, et al. The CC genotype in the T102C HTR2A polymorphism predicts relapse in individuals after alcohol treatment. J Psychiatr Res. 2013; 47(4): 527533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
189. Preuss, UW, Koller, G, Bondy, B, Bahlmann, M, Soyka, M. Impulsive traits and 5-HT2A receptor promoter polymorphism in alcohol dependents: possible association but no influence of personality disorders. Neuropsychobiology. 2001; 43(3): 186191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
190. Tsuang, HC, Chen, WJ, Lin, SH, et al. Impaired impulse control is associated with a 5-HT2A receptor polymorphism in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2013; 208(2): 105110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
191. Bjork, JM, Moeller, FG, Dougherty, DM, Swann, AC, Machado, MA, Hanis, CL. Serotonin 2a receptor T102C polymorphism and impaired impulse control. Am J Med Genet. 2002; 114(3): 336339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
192. Oquendo, MA, Russo, SA, Underwood, MD, et al. Higher postmortem prefrontal 5-HT2A receptor binding correlates with lifetime aggression in suicide. Biol Psychiatry. 2006; 59(3): 235243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
193. Dwivedi, Y, Mondal, AC, Payappagoudar, GV, Rizavi, HS. Differential regulation of serotonin (5HT)2A receptor mRNA and protein levels after single and repeated stress in rat brain: role in learned helplessness behavior. Neuropharmacology. 2005; 48(2): 204214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
194. Soloff, PH, Chiappetta, L, Mason, NS, Becker, C, Price, JC. Effects of serotonin-2A receptor binding and gender on personality traits and suicidal behavior in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2014; 222(3): 140148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
195. Meyer, JH, Wilson, AA, Rusjan, P, et al. Serotonin2A receptor binding potential in people with aggressive and violent behaviour. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2008; 33(6): 499508.Google ScholarPubMed
196. Rylands, AJ, Hinz, R, Jones, M, et al. Pre- and postsynaptic serotonergic differences in males with extreme levels of impulsive aggression without callous unemotional traits: a positron emission tomography study using (11)C-DASB and (11)C-MDL100907. Biol Psychiatry. 2012; 72(12): 10041011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
197. Chameau, P, van Hooft, JA. Serotonin 5-HT(3) receptors in the central nervous system. Cell Tissue Res. 2006; 326(2): 573581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
198. De Deurwaerdère, P, Moison, D, Navailles, S, Porras, G, Spampinato, U. Regionally and functionally distinct serotonin3 receptors control in vivo dopamine outflow in the rat nucleus accumbens. J Neurochem. 2005; 94(1): 140149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
199. Cervantes, MC, Delville, Y. Serotonin 5-HT1A and 5-HT3 receptors in an impulsive-aggressive phenotype. Behav Neurosci. 2009; 123(3): 589598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
200. Ricci, LA, Grimes, JM, Melloni, RH Jr.. Serotonin type 3 receptors modulate the aggression-stimulating effects of adolescent cocaine exposure in Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Behav Neurosci. 2004; 118(5): 10971110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
201. Ricci, LA, Knyshevski, I, Melloni, RH Jr.. Serotonin type 3 receptors stimulate offensive aggression in Syrian hamsters. Behav Brain Res. 2005; 156(1): 1929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
202. Rudissaar, R, Pruus, K, Skrebuhhova, T, Allikmets, L, Matto, V. Modulatory role of 5-HT3 receptors in mediation of apomorphine-induced aggressive behaviour in male rats. Behav Brain Res. 1999; 106(1–2): 9196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
203. McKenzie-Quirk, SD, Girasa, KA, Allan, AM, Miczek, KA. 5-HT(3) receptors, alcohol and aggressive behavior in mice. Behav Pharmacol. 2005; 16(3): 163169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
204. Sellers, EM, Toneatto, T, Romach, MK, Somer, GR, Sobell, LC, Sobell, MB. Clinical efficacy of the 5-HT3 antagonist ondansetron in alcohol abuse and dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1994; 18(4): 879885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
205. Johnson, BA, Ait-Daoud, N, Ma, JZ, Wang, Y. Ondansetron reduces mood disturbance among biologically predisposed, alcohol-dependent individuals. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003; 27(11): 17731779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
206. Myrick, H, Anton, RF, Li, X, Henderson, S, Randall, PK, Voronin, K. Effect of naltrexone and ondansetron on alcohol cue-induced activation of the ventral striatum in alcohol-dependent people. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008; 65(4): 466475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
207. Ducci, F, Enoch, MA, Yuan, Q, et al. HTR3B is associated with alcoholism with antisocial behavior and alpha EEG power—an intermediate phenotype for alcoholism and co-morbid behaviors. Alcohol. 2009; 43(1): 7384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
208. Melke, J, Westberg, L, Nilsson, S, et al. A polymorphism in the serotonin receptor 3A (HTR3A) gene and its association with harm avoidance in women. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003; 60(10): 10171023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
209. Gatt, JM, Williams, LM, Schofield, PR, et al. Impact of the HTR3A gene with early life trauma on emotional brain networks and depressed mood. Depress Anxiety. 2010; 27(8): 752759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
210. Iidaka, T, Ozaki, N, Matsumoto, A, et al. A variant C178T in the regulatory region of the serotonin receptor gene HTR3A modulates neural activation in the human amygdala. J Neurosci. 2005; 25(27): 64606466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
211. Schlüter, T, Winz, O, Henkel, K, et al. The impact of dopamine on aggression: an [18F]-FDOPA PET Study in healthy males. J Neurosci. 2013; 33(43): 1688916896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
212. Buckholtz, JW, Treadway, MT, Cowan, RL, et al. Mesolimbic dopamine reward system hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits. Nat Neurosci. 2010; 13(4): 419421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
213. Wagner, S, Baskaya, O, Anicker, NJ, Dahmen, N, Lieb, K, Tadic, A. The catechol o-methyltransferase (COMT) val(158)met polymorphism modulates the association of serious life events (SLE) and impulsive aggression in female patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD). Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010; 122(2): 110117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
214. Bhakta, SG, Zhang, JP, Malhotra, AK. The COMT Met158 allele and violence in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2012; 140(1–3): 192197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
215. Flory, JD, Xu, K, New, AS, Finch, T, Goldman, D, Siever, LJ. Irritable assault and variation in the COMT gene. Psychiatr Genet. 2007; 17(6): 344346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
216. Soyka, M, Zill, P, Koller, G, Samochowiec, A, Grzywacz, A, Preuss, UW. Val158Met COMT polymorphism and risk of aggression in alcohol dependence. Addict Biol. 2015; 20(1): 197204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
217. Vevera, J, Stopkova, R, Bes, M, et al. COMT polymorphisms in impulsively violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder. Neuro Endocrinol Lett. 2009; 30(6): 753756.Google ScholarPubMed
218. Hirata, Y, Zai, CC, Nowrouzi, B, Beitchman, JH, Kennedy, JL. Study of the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene with high aggression in children. Aggress Behav. 2013; 39(1): 4551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
219. Fresan, A, Camarena, B, Apiquian, R, Aguilar, A, Urraca, N, Nicolini, H. Association study of MAO-A and DRD4 genes in schizophrenic patients with aggressive behavior. Neuropsychobiology. 2007; 55(3–4): 171175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
220. Buchmann, AF, Zohsel, K, Blomeyer, D, et al. Interaction between prenatal stress and dopamine D4 receptor genotype in predicting aggression and cortisol levels in young adults. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2014; 231(16): 30893097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
221. Delville, Y, Mansour, KM, Ferris, CF. Serotonin blocks vasopressin-facilitated offensive aggression: interactions within the ventrolateral hypothalamus of golden hamsters. Physiol Behav. 1996; 59(4–5): 813816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
222. Ferris, CF, Melloni, RH Jr, Koppel, G, Perry, KW, Fuller, RW, Delville, Y. Vasopressin/serotonin interactions in the anterior hypothalamus control aggressive behavior in golden hamsters. J Neurosci. 1997; 17(11): 43314340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
223. Ferris, CF, Potegal, M. Vasopressin receptor blockade in the anterior hypothalamus suppresses aggression in hamsters. Physiol Behav. 1988; 44(2): 235239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
224. Bosch, OJ, Neumann, ID. Vasopressin released within the central amygdala promotes maternal aggression. Eur J Neurosci. 2010; 31(5): 883891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
225. Wersinger, SR, Caldwell, HK, Christiansen, M, Young, WS III. Disruption of the vasopressin 1b receptor gene impairs the attack component of aggressive behavior in mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2007; 6(7): 653660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
226. Wersinger, SR, Ginns, EI, O’Carroll, AM, Lolait, SJ, Young, WS III. Vasopressin V1b receptor knockout reduces aggressive behavior in male mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2002; 7(9): 975984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
227. Wersinger, SR, Caldwell, HK, Martinez, L, Gold, P, Hu, SB, Young, WS III. Vasopressin 1a receptor knockout mice have a subtle olfactory deficit but normal aggression. Genes Brain Behav. 2007; 6(6): 540551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
228. Fodor, A, Barsvari, B, Aliczki, M, et al. The effects of vasopressin deficiency on aggression and impulsiveness in male and female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014; 47: 141150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
229. Uzefovsky, F, Shalev, I, Israel, S, Knafo, A, Ebstein, RP. Vasopressin selectively impairs emotion recognition in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012; 37(4): 576580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
230. Guastella, AJ, Kenyon, AR, Alvares, GA, Carson, DS, Hickie, IB. Intranasal arginine vasopressin enhances the encoding of happy and angry faces in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 67(12): 12201222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
231. Lee, RJ, Coccaro, EF, Cremers, H, et al. A novel V1a receptor antagonist blocks vasopressin-induced changes in the CNS response to emotional stimuli: an fMRI study. Front Syst Neurosci. 2013; 7: 100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
232. Luppino, D, Moul, C, Hawes, DJ, Brennan, J, Dadds, MR. Association between a polymorphism of the vasopressin 1B receptor gene and aggression in children. Psychiatr Genet. 2014; 24(5): 185190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
233. Zai, CC, Muir, KE, Nowrouzi, B, et al. Possible genetic association between vasopressin receptor 1B and child aggression. Psychiatry Res. 2012; 200(2–3): 784788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
234. Vogel, F, Wagner, S, Baskaya, O, et al. Variable number of tandem repeat polymorphisms of the arginine vasopressin receptor 1A gene and impulsive aggression in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatr Genet. 2012; 22(2): 105106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
235. McBurnett, K, Lahey, BB, Rathouz, PJ, Loeber, R. Low salivary cortisol and persistent aggression in boys referred for disruptive behavior. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000; 57(1): 3843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
236. Popma, A, Vermeiren, R, Geluk, CA, et al. Cortisol moderates the relationship between testosterone and aggression in delinquent male adolescents. Biol Psychiatry. 2007; 61(3): 405411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
237. Kuepper, Y, Alexander, N, Osinsky, R, et al. Aggression–interactions of serotonin and testosterone in healthy men and women. Behav Brain Res. 2010; 206(1): 93100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
238. Welker, KM, Lozoya, E, Campbell, JA, Neumann, CS, Carre, JM. Testosterone, cortisol, and psychopathic traits in men and women. Physiol Behav. 2014; 129: 230236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
239. Denson, TF, Mehta, PH, Ho, TD. Endogenous testosterone and cortisol jointly influence reactive aggression in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013; 38(3): 416424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
240. Cima, M, Smeets, T, Jelicic, M. Self-reported trauma, cortisol levels, and aggression in psychopathic and non-psychopathic prison inmates. Biol Psychol. 2008; 78(1): 7586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
241. Derntl, B, Windischberger, C, Robinson, S, et al. Amygdala activity to fear and anger in healthy young males is associated with testosterone. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009; 34(5): 687693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
242. Hermans, EJ, Ramsey, NF, van Honk, J. Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2008; 63(3): 263270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
243. van Wingen, G, Mattern, C, Verkes, RJ, Buitelaar, J, Fernández, G. Testosterone reduces amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex coupling. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010; 35(1): 105113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
244. Goetz, SM, Tang, L, Thomason, ME, Diamond, MP, Hariri, AR, Carre, JM. Testosterone rapidly increases neural reactivity to threat in healthy men: a novel two-step pharmacological challenge paradigm. Biol Psychiatry. 2014; 76(4): 324331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
245. Grillo, CA, Risher, M, Macht, VA, et al. Repeated restraint stress-induced atrophy of glutamatergic pyramidal neurons and decreases in glutamatergic efflux in the rat amygdala are prevented by the antidepressant agomelatine. Neuroscience. 2015; 284: 430443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
246. Padival, MA, Blume, SR, Vantrease, JE, Rosenkranz, JA. Qualitatively different effect of repeated stress during adolescence on principal neuron morphology across lateral and basal nuclei of the rat amygdala. Neuroscience. 2015; 291: 128145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
247. Vyas, A, Mitra, R, Shankaranarayana Rao, BS, Chattarji, S. Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci. 2002; 22(15): 68106818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
248. Vyas, A, Bernal, S, Chattarji, S. Effects of chronic stress on dendritic arborization in the central and extended amygdala. Brain Res. 2003; 965(1-2): 290294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
249. Gilabert-Juan, J, Castillo-Gomez, E, Perez-Rando, M, Molto, MD, Nacher, J. Chronic stress induces changes in the structure of interneurons and in the expression of molecules related to neuronal structural plasticity and inhibitory neurotransmission in the amygdala of adult mice. Exp Neurol. 2011; 232(1): 3340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1 Amygdala: functional subdivisions and basic organization of flow of information. The amygdala is not a unitary structure, and consideration of its anatomical subdivisions and circuitry is essential. (a) One of the best characterized models of information flow and intra-amygdala circuitry involves the basolateral nuclear complex (which subsumes lateral and basal nuclei) and the central nuclear complex (which consists of a lateral [light red] and medial component [dark red]); these 2 complexes are considered the primary input and output components of this circuit, respectively. The basolateral complex receives cortical and thalamic sensory input. The lateral nucleus projects to the basal and central nuclei (lateral component); the basal nucleus projects to the central nucleus (medial component), as well as the intercalated masses (IM). The IMs are clusters of GABAergic inhibitory neurons, nestled between basolateral and central nuclei, which play a critical role in regulating central nucleus activity. The central nucleus projects to various subcortical regions, such as the hypothalamus, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), nucleus basalis (major site of acetylcholine [ACh] ascending projection neurons), and various brainstem regions that regulate neurotransmitter systems, autonomic function, innate psychomotor and visceromotor responses, and descending pain-regulation pathways. Central nucleus efferents are believed to consist of tonically active inhibitory projection neurons that activate their downstream targets when their activity is inhibited.30,31 (b)–(c) Studies of the amygdala in humans have typically considered it as a single functional unit, mainly due to its amorphous shape and relatively small size. Recent imaging studies have demonstrated the ability to differentiate the human amygdala in vivo into the 3 main nuclear complexes: basolateral, central (centromedial), and superficial (cortical).32

Figure 1

Figure 2 Prefrontal-Amygdala circuitry. Altered prefrontal amygdala circuitry has been implicated in the neurobiology of aggression. (a) Preclinical studies in primates and rodents have demonstrated that the portions of the prefrontal cortex most robustly interconnected, anatomically, with the amygdala include 2 basic regions: the posterior extent of the orbitofrontal cortex (pOFC) and the caudal portions of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), roughly corresponding to the subgenual (sg)ACC and pregenual (pg)ACC. Anatomical connectivity between the amygdala and PFC, in general, decreases as it moves toward the dorsal and lateral aspects of the PFC (not depicted in this figure). Although there is significant bilateral anatomical connectivity between the amygdala and both the ACC and pOFC, studies in primates have also demonstrated relatively greater ACC-to-amygdala projections than amygdala-to-ACC projections; the pOFC, on the other hand, receives a greater number of projections from the amygdala compared to projections the pOFC sends to the amygdala. This has been considered to signify that the ACC is more of a sender of amygdala-PFC projections, whereas, the pOFC is more of a receiver.49,50 (b) These structural data in primates are broadly consistent with intrinsic functional connectivity studies of the amygdala complex in humans, although the latter provides, of course, relatively less anatomical resolution. As illustrated in (b), the subgenual/orbital portion of the medial PFC exhibits positive functional connectivity (red) with the basolateral amygdala complex, which suggests a predominantly feed-forward relationship. The pregenual medial PFC, on the other hand, exhibits negative functional connectivity with the basolateral amygdala complex (blue), which indicates a feed-back role.52 There is a general absence of functional connectivity with the more dorsal and lateral (not shown) PFC, which is similar to primate structural findings. (c)–(d) There are important anatomical differences in the termination patterns of projections from the OFC and ACC to the amygdala in the primate.51 Projection neurons from the ACC terminate broadly throughout the basal nucleus (as well as other amygdala nuclei), whereas OFC efferents are relatively unique in that they terminate among the intercalated masses or IMs (clusters of GABAergic inhibitory neurons that regulate central nucleus activity), as well as the central nucleus itself. Therefore, the pOFC may act as a direct arbiter of central nucleus activity, and therefore, determine the extent to which a stimulus representation is infused with a visceral/autonomic or “somatic” component. The ACC, on the other hand, may influence more broadly how sensory inputs to the amygdala are processed. It remains unclear how pregenual and subgenual portions of the ACC may differ in this respect, however.