Elsevier

Behavioural Brain Research

Volume 317, 15 January 2017, Pages 163-178
Behavioural Brain Research

Research report
The impact of a junk-food diet during development on ‘wanting’ and ‘liking’

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.041Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Junk-food (JF) exposure during development reduces weight gain in two strains of rats.

  • JF-induced weight gain has opposite effects on cue attraction in males and females.

  • JF gainers work harder for cues and are more attracted to a JF context.

  • JF exposure in Long-Evans, but not Sprague-Dawley rats, blunts sucrose ‘liking’.

  • JF exposure reduces anxiety-like behavior in males, but not females.

Abstract

The global increase in obesity rates has been tied to the rise in junk-food availability and consumption. Increasingly, children are exposed to a junk-food diet during gestation and early development. Excessive consumption of junk-food during this period may negatively impact the development of brain motivation and reward pathways. In this study we investigated the effects of a chronic junk-food diet throughout development on cue-motivated behavior (‘wanting’), hedonic ‘liking’ for sweet tastes, as well as anxiety and weight gain in male and female Long-Evans (LE) and Sprague-Dawley (SD) rats. Here we found that chronic exposure to a junk-food diet resulted in large individual differences in weight gain (gainers and non-gainers) despite resulting in stunted growth as compared to chow-fed controls. Behaviorally, junk-food exposure attenuated conditioned approach (autoshaping) in females, particularly in non-gainers. In contrast, junk-food exposed rats that gained the most weight were willing to work harder for access to a food cue (conditioned reinforcement), and were more attracted to a junk-food context (conditioned place preference) than non-gainers. Hedonic ‘liking’ reactions (taste reactivity) were severely blunted in LE, but not SD rats, and ‘liking’ for sucrose negatively correlated with greater weight gain. Finally, junk-food exposure reduced anxiety-like behavior (elevated plus maze) in males but not females. These results suggest that junk-food exposure during development may give rise to dissociable differences in ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ neural systems that do not depend on weight gain and may not be detected through Body Mass Index monitoring alone.

Introduction

Obesity is a global health risk, and the rapid escalation of its prevalence suggests shifting environmental factors may have a role in its growth. As of 2012, over 15% of children and over 30% of adults in the United States are obese, while another 30% of the population is overweight [1]. These numbers are representative of a growing obesity epidemic [1], [2], [3]. The growing accessibility of inexpensive processed foods and their increasingly pervasive advertising may play a role in this alarming trend [4], [5]. Many of these processed foods are saturated with sugar, salt, and fat. Yet they lack adequate protein and other nutrients that are important for day-to-day health and normal growth and development, categorizing them as “junk-food”. In countries with high and rising obesity rates, daily food intake is not exclusively driven by hunger or energy demand. It is suggested that for some individuals, the increased palatability and accessibility of junk-food has seized neural reward and motivation mechanisms and turned food-seeking into errant food craving, which may lead to diet-induced obesity [6], [7].

When a new food is first ingested, its sensory qualities may trigger sensations of hedonic pleasure and ‘liking’, which in turn promote ‘wanting’ to consume that food again [6], [8]. With repeated exposure, however, the environmental cues associated with the junk-food may gain more motivating power and incentive value. The salience of cues associated with food is facilitated through activity in mesocorticolimbic systems, which makes rewards and their cues desired and ‘wanted’ [9], [10], [11]. The neural systems for ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ typically function in close synchrony, but data show that they can be changed independently. For example, with repeated consumption of a reward, such as palatable junk-food, ‘wanting’ becomes sensitized [11], [12]. Sensitization of ‘wanting’ was first described in the ‘incentive sensitization’ theory of addiction, and can result in a dissociation of ‘wanting’ and ‘liking’ that leads to strong feelings of desire for particular rewards and their cues, despite no increase and sometimes a reduction in ‘liking’ [13]. Although it was initially applied to drugs and their cues, recent evidence suggests this theory also applies to food cues. Food cues can play a similar role by triggering visual attention and enhancing the desire to eat [14], [15], [16], [17], particularly in obese individuals who might be hyper-responsive to the motivational properties of these cues [18], [19], [20].

However, susceptibility to (incentive) sensitization appears to show a large degree of individual variation, with marked sex differences [21]. For example, there is evidence for individual variation in the level of attraction and motivation to junk-food cues [12], [22], [23]. In particular, we recently demonstrated that animals that gained excessive weight on a junk-food diet (gainers) displayed greater cue-induced approach to food cues even before gaining access to the diet [12], and were also more willing to work for the presentation of those cues (conditioned reinforcement) after obesity onset. However, many of these studies were carried out in adults. The current ease of access and high palatability of these foods means that exposure to a junk-food diet may begin as early as childhood or even prior to birth through the mother’s diet.

Childhood obesity has been implicated as a cofactor in a number of lifetime diseases such as depression, anxiety, diabetes, elevated blood pressure, orthopedic problems, and pulmonary complications [24], [25], [26], and has been associated with early mortality [27], [28]. Previous studies have shown that a mother’s diet during pregnancy alters the protein make-up of the offspring’s cerebral cortex despite cross-fostering [29], while also producing changes in dopaminergic activity [30]. Developmental perspectives on the obesity epidemic are necessary to understand the increasing prevalence of childhood obesity across generations [1], [25], and dissociations between ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ could have a lasting impact when occurring within the plastic neural networks of a maturing brain. However, it is currently unclear whether overconsumption of junk-food is related to distortions of either ‘liking’ or ‘wanting’, or both, when exposure begins prenatally.

Here we examined the effect of lifetime exposure to junk-food on ‘wanting’ by measuring the degree to which food cues 1) elicit approach (autoshaping), 2) reinforce operant responding (conditioned reinforcement), and 3) by determining the attraction of a junk-food paired context (conditioned place preference). We also measured the impact of lifetime exposure to a junk-food diet on hedonic orofacial ‘liking’ reactions, using taste reactivity measures [31] in response to sucrose. In addition, since anxiety is often associated with increased consumption of fatty-sugary foods [32], we also evaluated individual differences in the impact of junk-food on levels of anxiety-like behavior using the elevated plus maze. Finally there are marked sex differences in the motivation for food [33], [34]. Recent findings also show strain and sex differences for spatial learning [35], [36], behavior toward unfamiliar foods [37] and metabolic responses [38]. Therefore measures of ‘wanting’, ‘liking’ and anxiety were determined in males vs. females, across two strains of rats, Long-Evans and Sprague-Dawley.

Section snippets

Subjects

Long-Evans (LE) and Sprague-Dawley (SD) rats were bred in-house from breeding pairs purchased from both Harlan and Charles River. Rats were housed on a 12:12 h reverse light/dark cycle and had ad-lib access to food and water unless stated otherwise. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee for Wesleyan University.

Diet

Adult male and female rats were placed on either a standard chow and junk-food (JF) or a control diet (C; Teklad Rodent Lab Diet 2018 in pellet

Weight gain and food intake

There were no differences in weight on postnatal day (PND) 7 between offspring born to mothers maintained on a control (C; standard chow) or junk-food (JF) diet during gestation and weaning (Day 7: F(1,87) = 1.669, p = 0.200; Fig. 1B). However, although both groups gained weight over time (Effect of Day: F(1,86) = 3343.184, p = 0.000), they did so at different rates (Day by Diet: F(1,86) = 41.225, p = 0.000). Surprisingly, by PND 21, body weight was on average 15% lower in offspring from the junk-food

Developmental effects of a prenatal and lifetime junk-food diet

This study was conducted to investigate the influence of lifetime exposure to a junk-food diet on motivated ‘wanting’, hedonic sucrose ‘liking’, and anxiety in Long-Evans (LE) and Sprague-Dawley (SD) rats of both sexes. A previous study using the same junk-food diet recipe found that while some rats gained excessive weight on the diet, others maintained a body weight similar to animals that ate standard chow; these rats were termed “gainers” and “non-gainers” [12]. In contrast, the junk-food

Conclusion

The current study examines the impact of a lifetime exposure to a junk-food diet in male and female rats of two outbred strains. Our results highlight how an unhealthy highly palatable diet that induces obesity in adults, instead results in stunted weight gain during development. If the same is true in humans, this could result in diet-induced health issues that may not be detected by measures such as the body mass index (BMI), which is currently used as the primary diagnosis tool for childhood

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr Carrie Ferrario for helpful comments on the manuscript, along with Melanie Schaffler, Caroline Mead, Louise Lyu, Cornelia Channing, Olivia Lofaro, Rebecca Tom, Jeremy Levit, Adam Fischer and Charlotte Freeland for their technical assistance. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References (81)

  • J.B. Becker

    Sexual differentiation of motivation: a novel mechanism?

    Horm. Behav.

    (2009)
  • A.C. Reichelt et al.

    Differential motivational profiles following adolescent sucrose access in male and female rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2016)
  • K.N. Abbott et al.

    Sex-specific effects of daily exposure to sucrose on spatial memory performance in male and female rats, and implications for estrous cycle stage

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2016)
  • K. Modlinska et al.

    Food neophobia in wild and laboratory rats (multi-strain comparison)

    Behav. Process.

    (2015)
  • T.E. Robinson et al.

    On the motivational properties of reward cues: individual differences

    Neuropharmacology

    (2014)
  • M.J.F. Robinson et al.

    Central but not peripheral beta-adrenergic antagonism blocks reconsolidation for a morphine place preference

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2007)
  • K.C. Berridge

    Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2000)
  • M.J.F. Robinson et al.

    Effects of anisomycin on consolidation and reconsolidation of a morphine-conditioned place preference

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2007)
  • A.A.M. da Silva et al.

    Low protein diet during gestation and lactation increases food reward seeking but does not modify sucrose taste reactivity in adult female rats

    Int. J. Dev. Neurosci.

    (2016)
  • W. Warneke et al.

    The impact of cafeteria diet feeding on physiology and anxiety-related behaviour in male and female Sprague-Dawley rats of different ages

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (2014)
  • A.P. Starling et al.

    Associations of maternal BMI and gestational weight gain with neonatal adiposity in the Healthy Start study

    Am. J. Clin. Nutr.

    (2015)
  • P. Anselme et al.

    Reward uncertainty enhances incentive salience attribution as sign-tracking

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2013)
  • T.E. Robinson et al.

    Dissociating the predictive and incentive motivational properties of reward-related cues through the study of individual differences

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • T.C. Napier et al.

    Using conditioned place preference to identify relapse prevention medications

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2013)
  • M.J.F. Robinson et al.

    Instant transformation of learned repulsion into motivational wanting

    Curr. Biol.

    (2013)
  • R. Soussignan et al.

    Orofacial reactivity to the sight and smell of food stimuli. Evidence for anticipatory liking related to food reward cues in overweight children

    Appetite

    (2012)
  • A. Sasaki et al.

    Maternal high-fat diet alters anxiety behavior and glucocorticoid signaling in adolescent offspring

    Neuroscience

    (2014)
  • S. Mora et al.

    Effects of the estrous cycle and ovarian hormones on behavioral indices of anxiety in female rats

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (1996)
  • F.K. Marcondes et al.

    Estrous cycle influences the response of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2001)
  • K.K. Pitchers et al.

    Individual variation in the propensity to attribute incentive salience to a food cue: influence of sex

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2015)
  • C.L. Ogden et al.

    Trends in obesity prevalence among children and adolescents in the United States, 1988–1994 through 2013–2014

    JAMA

    (2016)
  • S.L. Gortmaker et al.

    Increasing pediatric obesity in the United States

    Am. J. Dis. Child.

    (1987)
  • A.A. Hedley et al.

    Prevalence of overweight and obesity among US children, adolescents, and adults, 1999–2002

    JAMA

    (2004)
  • K.S. Collison et al.

    Sugar-sweetened carbonated beverage consumption correlates with BMI, waist circumference, and poor dietary choices in school children

    BMC Public Health

    (2010)
  • S.D. Leary et al.

    Behavioural early-life exposures and body composition at age 15 years

    Nutr. Diabetes

    (2015)
  • H.-R. Berthoud

    The neurobiology of food intake in an obesogenic environment

    Proc. Nutr. Soc.

    (2012)
  • M.J.F. Robinson et al.

    Incentive salience in addiction and over-consumption

  • M.J.F. Robinson et al.

    Roles of wanting and liking in motivating behavior: gambling, food, and drug addictions

  • M.J.F. Robinson et al.

    Individual differences in cue-induced motivation and striatal systems in rats susceptible to diet-Induced obesity

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (2015)
  • K.J. Doolan et al.

    Visual attention to food cues in obesity: an eye-tracking study

    Obesity

    (2014)
  • Cited by (14)

    • Brain regulation of hunger and motivation: The case for integrating homeostatic and hedonic concepts and its implications for obesity and addiction

      2022, Appetite
      Citation Excerpt :

      Causal studies using rodents suggest that diet induced obesity downregulates D2 receptor expression in striatal regions that resembles findings reported in humans (Johnson & Kenny, 2010; Morales & Berridge, 2020; Robinson et al., 2015; Stice, Yokum, Blum, & Bohon, 2010). Prolonged exposure to a junk-food diet may also blunt hedonic ‘liking’ reactions in some female rats (Lesser, Arroyo-Ramirez, Mi, & Robinson, 2017), and that prolonged exposure to high-sugar diets can dysregulate epigenetic mechanisms that result in decreased sweet taste processing and promote overconsumption and weight gain (May, Rosander, Gottfried, Dennis, & Dus, 2020; Vaziri et al., 2020). Obesity is also associated with alterations to additional brain systems involved in regulating food intake, including glutamate, AgRP, mu-opioids, and endocannabinoids within distributed sites in hypothalamus, striatum, and ventral tegmental area (Barnes, Holmes, Primeaux, York, & Bray, 2006; Bourdy et al., 2021; Briggs, Enriori, Lemus, Cowley, & Andrews, 2010; Johnson & Kenny, 2010; M.; Robinson et al., 2015; Rossi et al., 2019).

    • Chronic exposure to cafeteria-style diet in rats alters sweet taste preference and reduces motivation for, but not ‘liking’ of sucrose

      2022, Appetite
      Citation Excerpt :

      A recent study in rats found that 10 weeks access to high-fat diet beginning at weaning did not alter hedonic orofacial responses to percentage 0.1M or 1.0M sucrose, but reduced motivation to work for sweet food rewards (Arcego et al., 2020). Similarly, Lesser et al. (2017) showed that chronic exposure to a high-fat, high-sugar ‘junk-food’ diet (a purified mix of cookies, potato chips, peanut butter, chocolate powder and rat chow) suppressed approach behaviour to a food-paired cue but did not alter hedonic orofacial responses to 1, 3 and 9% sucrose in Sprague-Dawley rats. An intriguing strain difference was observed, with junk-food reducing both ‘wanting’ and ‘liking’ in Long-Evans, relative to Sprague-Dawley rats.

    • The cafeteria diet: A standardized protocol and its effects on behavior

      2021, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition, these “mashed CAF pellets” retain the advantage of having the taste of junk food. Some researchers have thus started to use them as an alternative to both HF/HS diet and CAF diet (e.g. de la Garza et al., 2019; Derman and Ferrario, 2018; Estadella et al., 2004; Lesser et al., 2017; Schimidt et al., 2018). Notwithstanding this good alternative solution, we have not included “mashed CAF pellets” studies in this review, because real human products improve the construct validity of the CAF diet, for we, as a society, do not eat pellets.

    • Reward uncertainty attributes incentive value to reward proximal cues, while amphetamine sensitization reverts attention to more predictive reward distal cues

      2020, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Wesleyan University. All testing (sensitization, training) was conducted in Med Associates Inc. modular test chambers (25.8 × 32.2 × 33.2 cm) with metal bar floors, two modular front and back walls and two plexiglass walls, as previously described (Lesser et al., 2017). Each chamber was equipped with two retractable levers located on the front wall of the chamber, either side of a recessed magazine dish, which delivered 45 mg sucrose pellets (TestDiet, St. Louis, MO, USA), and was equipped with an infrared beam and sensor to record head entries.

    • Distinguishing between predictive and incentive value of uncertain gambling-like cues in a Pavlovian autoshaping task

      2019, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The two groups differed by reward condition (Certain: 100%-1 or Uncertain: 50%-1-2-3) according to the probability and magnitude of reward delivery per CS trial during autoshaping (Fig. 1B). All testing was conducted in Med Associates Inc.™ modular test chambers (25.8 × 32.2 × 33.2 cm) with metal bar floors, two modular front and back walls and two plexiglass walls, as previously described [30]. Each chamber was equipped with two retractable levers located on the front wall of the chamber, either side of a recessed magazine dish, which delivered 45 mg sucrose pellets (TestDiet, St. Louis, MO, USA), and was equipped with an infrared beam and sensor to record head entries.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Present address: Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA.

    View full text