Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 57, Issue 3, December 2011, Pages 711-717
Appetite

Research report
Evidence that ‘food addiction’ is a valid phenotype of obesity

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.017Get rights and content

Abstract

There is growing evidence of ‘food addiction’ (FA) in sugar- and fat-bingeing animals. The purpose of this study was to investigate the legitimacy of this disorder in the human condition. It was also our intention to extend the validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS) – the first tool developed to identify individuals with addictive tendencies towards food. Using a sample of obese adults (aged 25–45 years), and a case–control methodology, we focused our assessments on three domains relevant to the characterization of conventional substance-dependence disorders: clinical co-morbidities, psychological risk factors, and abnormal motivation for the addictive substance. Results were strongly supportive of the FA construct and validation of the YFAS. Those who met the diagnostic criteria for FA had a significantly greater co-morbidity with Binge Eating Disorder, depression, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder compared to their age- and weight-equivalent counterparts. Those with FA were also more impulsive and displayed greater emotional reactivity than obese controls. They also displayed greater food cravings and the tendency to ‘self-soothe’ with food. These findings advance the quest to identify clinically relevant subtypes of obesity that may possess different vulnerabilities to environmental risk factors, and thereby could inform more personalized treatment approaches for those who struggle with overeating and weight gain.

Highlights

► Validation for the Yale Food Addiction Scale to identify those with addictive tendencies to food. ► Obese food addicts show higher comorbidity with binge eating, depression and ADHD than controls. ► Food addicts are more impulsive and show more addictive personality traits than controls. ► Our findings have demonstrated strong parallels between food and substance abuse.

Introduction

Although the concept of food addiction (FA) is widely accepted among the general public – as seen by the many 12-Step treatment programs established for those who struggle with overeating – it has only recently gained some credibility in the scientific community. For example, there has been a steeply exponential increase in the number of academic publications relating to this putative condition over the past 5 years (Gearhardt, Davis, Kuschner, & Brownell, in press).1 During the same period, there has also been a noticeable shift in perspective towards the view that addictions should be re-framed as unusually strong and maladaptive desires for pleasure (or reduction of distress), irrespective of the source of the reward (Hoebel, Avena, Bocarsly, & Rada, 2009). The current interest in FA as a clinically valid construct has undoubtedly been fostered by the worldwide shift in body mass index (BMI) towards the high end of the distribution, and by the concomitant per capita increase in daily calorie intake (Apovian, 2010).

The foods we most prefer to eat are those high in fat, sugar, and salt – substances which were generally found in small amounts, and intermittently, in the diets of our evolutionary ancestors (Armelagos, 2010). However, in the quantities we currently consume these hyper-palatable foods they appear to have an abuse potential similar to conventional addictive drugs (Gearhardt et al., in press, Kenny, 2011, Spring et al., 2008). Potent sweeteners such as high fructose corn syrup (HFCS) are particularly influential because they have been added voluminously to a large variety of processed foods including soft drinks, baked goods, and cereals (Bray, 2008). Indeed, our consumption of this monosaccharide has increased considerably in a few generations, from about 4% to 12% of our daily caloric intake (Vos, Kimmons, Gillespie, Welsh, & Blanck, 2008). Moreover, the special physiological properties of fructose enhance its similarity to other addictive drugs. The most direct parallels are seen between fructose and alcohol because the two are biochemically congruent – ethanol is simply the fermented byproduct of fructose (Lustig, 2010). Different from glucose, fructose blunts leptin signaling thereby promoting sensations of hunger and creating a desire for consumption independent of energy needs (Li, Li, Kong, & Hu, 2010). This seems to occur because fructose bypasses the insulin-driven satiety system. In other words, while glucose stimulates the release of insulin, decreasing the desire to eat, fructose has this effect only to a very weak degree. Consequently, when eaten in abundance, fructose can result in biological changes that promote overconsumption and encourage further problematic use (Bocarsly, Powell, Avena, & Hoebel, 2010).

Few would dispute that some individuals display an apparent ‘loss of control’ over food intake that is similar to the behaviour of those who abuse drugs and alcohol. We have previously argued that the compulsive overeating seen in Binge Eating Disorder (BED) has compelling parallels to conventional drug addictions based on their comparable clinical features, the biological mechanisms they have in common, and evidence of a shared diathesis (Davis & Carter, 2009). This perspective also aligns with some qualitative evidence that a high proportion of women diagnosed with BED endorse the criteria for drug dependence when the word ‘substance’ refers to binge eating instead of drugs (Cassin & von Ranson, 2007).

While early discussions described the mood enhancing effects and pronounced cravings that compulsive overeating has in common with drug abuse (Rogers & Smit, 2000), evidence of their biological parallels is more recent, and has relied largely on well-controlled animal research. Rodent models of FA have typically used behavioural paradigms based on analogues of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual [DSM-IV-TR] (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) criteria for drug dependence. For example, escalation of intake is used as a marker of “taking the substance in larger amounts than was intended” (Corwin & Grigson, 2009). There is now reliable evidence that rats fed an intermittent diet of sugar, develop a pattern of copious consumption resembling human cases of binge eating (Avena & Hoebel, 2003).

These studies show that a sugar-enhanced diet increases daily food intake over time, and that following its removal, the animals show aggression, anxiety, teeth-chattering, and head-shaking – all symptoms associated with withdrawal from drugs like heroin. Similar results have been found when animals were given high-fat (Lutter & Nestler, 2009) or other highly palatable diets (Johnson & Kenny, 2010). Female rats prone to binge eating were also found to tolerate significantly higher levels of foot shock for access to Oreo cookies than their binge-resistant counterparts, confirming their abnormal motivation for sweet and fatty foods (Oswald, Murdaugh, King, & Boggiano, 2011). In addition, while numerous studies have found behavioural and consummatory cross-sensitization from one addictive drug to another (Avena, Rada, & Hoebel, 2008) – and between drugs and stress (Covington & Miczek, 2001) – there is now good evidence that sugar intake also cross-sensitizes to drugs of abuse, and vice versa (Avena et al., 2008).2

Despite the robust evidence of sugar/fat dependence in rodents, there are few parallel findings in human research. Some notable exceptions are the work of Pelchat (2009) who identified brain areas responsive to both food and drug cravings. Other important research has demonstrated the abuse potential of sugar, based on laboratory indicators similar to those used to test the abuse liability of drugs (Spring et al., 2008). Researchers at Yale University have recently moved the field forward by developing a measure to operationalize human cases of FA using the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for substance dependence (Gearhardt, Corbin, & Brownell, 2009). Their preliminary evidence suggests that the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS) has high convergent validity with other measures of eating pathology – especially binge eating – and may therefore be a useful tool to identify those with addictive tendencies towards food. In a recent neuroimaging study, they also found that YFAS scores correlated with neural activation in brain regions that play a role in the experience of cravings, and that high scorers exhibited activation patterns associated with reduced inhibitory control (Gearhardt et al., 2011). However, this study was somewhat limited by a small all-female sample, and because only two participants met the diagnostic criteria for FA.

The purpose of the present study was to provide further support for the FA construct, and to extend the validation of the YFAS by using a non-clinical sample of obese adults. We focused our assessments on three domains relevant to the characterization of conventional addiction disorders: clinical co-morbidities, psychological risk factors, and abnormal motivation for the addictive substance. In the first instance, we anticipated a higher prevalence of BED in those meeting criteria for FA, as well as more severe symptoms of depression and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder since both these latter conditions have strong co-morbid links to drug abuse (Carpentier et al., 2011, de los Cobos et al., 2011, Fuemmeler et al., 2007). In addition, almost a decade of research has established strong links between ADHD and obesity, both in adults, and in children and adolescents, and it appears that this relationship is not attributable to socio-demographic factors that influence people's dietary patterns and opportunity for physical activity (see Davis, 2010). We also expected food addicts – like drug addicts – to be more impulsive and to have higher scores on a measure of addictive personality traits (e.g. Gullo, Ward, Dawe, Powell, & Jackson, 2011). Finally, given the high incentive salience of ‘drugs’ to the drug addict (George & Koob, 2010), we hypothesized that food addicts would also report a greater hedonic motivation for food and be more likely to overeat in response to emotional and environmental triggers in the absence of hunger, when compared to their non-FA counterparts.

Section snippets

Participants

Seventy-two obese adult women (n = 49) and men (n = 23) between the ages of 25 and 46 years took part in the study. Participants were required to be fluent in English and to have lived in North America for at least 5 years prior to their enrolment. All female participants were also required to be pre-menopausal as identified by the self-reporting of regular menstrual cycles, and not to have had a pregnancy within the previous 6 months. Exclusion criteria included a current diagnosis of any

Food addiction diagnosis

According to the YFAS diagnostic scoring procedure, which is based on the DSM-IV criteria for substance dependence, 18 adults (female = 13; male = 5) were classified as food addicts.

Demographic characteristics: Compared to their non-food addict counterparts (female = 36; male = 18), the group of food addicts did not differ significantly in age or BMI. Nor was there a different gender ratio, ethnic composition, or education level between the two groups (see Table 1).

Clinical features: The groups were

Discussion

Our findings provide very good validation for the YFAS and its ability to identify individuals with addictive tendencies towards food. In so doing, they strongly reinforce the view that FA is a classifiable condition with clinical symptomatology and a psycho-behavioural profile similar to conventional drug abuse disorders. These results also deliver much needed human support for the growing evidence of addiction in sugar- and fat-bingeing rats (Avena, 2010). Importantly, our findings are not

References (78)

  • C. Davis et al.

    Compulsive overeating as an addiction disorder. A review of theory and evidence

    Appetite

    (2009)
  • L. Fattore et al.

    The endocannabinoid system and nondrug rewarding behaviours

    Experimental Neurology

    (2010)
  • A.N. Gearhardt et al.

    Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale

    Appetite

    (2009)
  • O. George et al.

    Individual differences in prefrontal cortex function and the transition from drug use to drug dependence

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2010)
  • M.J. Gullo et al.

    Support for a two-factor model of impulsivity and hazardous substance use in British and Australian young adults

    Journal of Research in Personality

    (2011)
  • P.J. Kenny

    Reward mechanisms in obesity. New insights and future directions

    Neuron

    (2011)
  • M.R. Lowe et al.

    The Power of Food Scale. A new measure of the psychological influence of the food environment

    Appetite

    (2009)
  • M. Lutter et al.

    Homeostatic and hedonic signals interact in the regulation of food intake

    The Journal of Nutrtition

    (2009)
  • R.H. Lustig

    Fructose. Metabolic, hedonic, and societal parallels with ethanol

    Journal of the American Dietetic Association

    (2010)
  • M.P. McCabe et al.

    Are there different sociocultural influences on body image and body change strategies for overweight adolescent boys and girls?

    Eating Behaviors

    (2010)
  • M.J. Meaney et al.

    Environmental regulation of the development of mesolimbic dopamine systems. A neurobiological mechanism for vulnerability to drug abuse?

    Psychoneuroenocrinology

    (2002)
  • A. Odum

    Delay discounting. Trait variable?

    Behavioural Processes

    (2011)
  • M.L. Pelchat

    Food cravings in young and elderly adults

    Appetite

    (1997)
  • M.L. Pelchat

    Food addiction in humans

    Journal of Nutrition

    (2009)
  • B.M. Popkin

    Recent dynamics suggest selected countries catching up to US obesity

    American Journal of Clinical Nutrition

    (2010)
  • P.J. Rogers et al.

    Craving and food addiction. A critical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective

    Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior

    (2000)
  • D.G. Schlundt et al.

    The Eating Behavior Questionnaire predicts dietary fat intake in African American women

    Journal of the American Dietetic Association

    (2003)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual. Version IV-TR

    (2000)
  • G.J. Armelagos

    The omnivore's dilemma. The evolution of the brain and the determinants of food choice

    Journal of Anthropological Research

    (2010)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Manual for the beck depression inventory (BDI-II)

    (1996)
  • J.B. Becker et al.

    Gender differences in the behavioral responses to cocaine and amphetamine. Implications for mechanisms mediating gender differences in drug abuse

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2001)
  • D.M. Blumenthal et al.

    Neurobiology of food addiction

    Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition and Metabolic Care

    (2010)
  • G.A. Bray

    Fructose. Should we worry?

    International Journal of Obesity

    (2008)
  • P.J. Carpentier et al.

    Influence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder on opioid dependence severity and psychiatric comorbidity in chronic methadone-maintained patients

    European Addiction Research

    (2011)
  • D. Clarke

    Gambling and the trait of addiction in a sample of New Zealand university students

    New Zealand Journal of Psychology

    (2003)
  • C.K. Conners et al.

    Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scales

    (1999)
  • H.E. Covington et al.

    Repeated social defeat stress, cocaine or morphine. Effects on behavioral sensitization and intravenous cocaine self-administration “binges”

    Psychopharmacology (Berlin)

    (2001)
  • J.P. Crean et al.

    Reward discounting as a measure of impulsive behavior in a psychiatric outpatient population

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (2000)
  • C. Davis

    Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Associations with overeating and obesity

    Current Psychiatry Reports

    (2010)
  • Cited by (382)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text