Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 54, Issue 1, February 2010, Pages 84-92
Appetite

Research report
Decision-making impairments in women with binge eating disorder

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.010Get rights and content

Abstract

Even though eating is frequently driven by overindulgence and reward rather than by energy balance, few studies so far have analyzed decision-making processes and disturbances in feedback processing in women with binge eating disorder (BED). In an experimental study, 17 women with BED (DSM-IV) and 18 overweight healthy controls (HC) were compared in the game of dice task (GDT). This task assesses decision-making under risk with explicit rules for gains and losses. Additionally, differences in dispositional activation of the behavior inhibition and behavior approach system as well as cognitive flexibility were measured. Main results revealed that women with BED make risky decisions significantly more often than HC. Moreover, they show impaired capacities to advantageously utilize feedback processing. Even though these deficits were not related to disease-specific variables, they may be important for the daily decision-making behavior of women with BED, thus being relevant as a maintenance factor for the disorder.

Introduction

Uncontrolled eating in the form of binge attacks is the core feature of women suffering from binge eating disorder (BED). Several studies indicate that the consequences of binge eating include overweight and obesity, medical (Bulik et al., 2002, Johnson et al., 2001) and psychological sequelae (Hudson, Hiripi, Pope, & Kessler, 2007) and an increased mortality risk (Fichter, Quadflieg, & Hedlund, 2008). Research so far has mostly focused on the role of negative emotions (Arnow et al., 1995, Hilbert and Tuschen-Caffier, 2007, Stein et al., 2007, Svaldi et al., in press, Telch and Agras, 1996) and over evaluation of weight and shape (Grilo et al., 2008a, Masheb and Grilo, 2008, Mond et al., 2007, Svaldi et al., 2009) in the onset and maintenance of binge attacks in BED. Comparably, neuropsychological paradigms have been neglected or have mainly focused on subclinical eating disorders, anorexia nervosa (AN), bulimia nervosa (BN) and obesity.

Given the high availability of palatable food, gluttonous eating may not only be a response to powerful physiological drives. Specifically, on several occasions, resisting omnipresent temptations may require skills such as behavior inhibition, attention shift and delay of gratification. Such skills are supposed to be associated with prefrontal cortex regions and distinct subcortical structures (so-called fronto-striatal loops) that mediate executive functions (Lie et al., 2006, Markela-Lerenc et al., 2004, Masterman and Cummings, 1997, Petrides and Pandya, 2002, Robbins, 1996). For example, Nederkoorn, Van Eijs, and Jansen (2004) used the stop signal task (Logan, Schachar, & Tannock, 1997) to test behavior inhibition in restrained eaters. In the stop signal task, participants have to respond as fast as possible in a choice reaction time task, unless a stop signal is presented. In this case, participants have to inhibit their response. Nederkoorn et al. (2004) found restrained eaters to be significantly worse in the inhibition of non-food-related motor responses. Similarly, obese women (Nederkoorn, Smulders, Havermans, Roefs, & Jansen, 2006) and obese children (Nederkoorn, Braet, Van Eijs, Tanghe, & Jansen, 2006) are affected by less adequate response inhibition, although null results for AN and BN have been reported as well (e.g., Claes, Nederkoorn, Vandereycken, Guerrieri, & Vertommen, 2006).

In neuropsychological research, another task to evaluate intentional motor inhibition is the Go/No-Go task (Nigg, 2000). In this task, participants are required to press a key whenever a target stimulus is presented. At the same time, they have to refrain from pressing the key whenever a non-target stimulus is presented. Rosval et al. (2006) found women with BN and women with AN binge/purging type to display significantly more errors than women with AN restricting type and healthy controls. Mobbs, Van der Linden, d’Acremont, and Perroud (2008) assessed inhibitory control in women with BN and healthy controls by use of an adapted Go/No-Go task which included stimuli related to the body and food. Again, participants with BN expressed inhibition problems, especially on food-related targets.

Against the background of the widespread accessibility of high caloric, tasty food, healthy nutrition is, to a large degree, dependent on conscious decisions with regard to food choices, quantity and frequency of meals. A task recognized to detect decision-making impairments is the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) which requires subjects to choose from various alternatives (card decks) without having any information about the outcome (gains or losses). Patients with AN (Cavedini et al., 2004), patients with BN (Boeka & Lokken, 2006) as well as obese individuals (Davis, Levitan, Muglia, Bewell, & Kennedy, 2004) expressed disadvantageous decision-making patterns in this task. However, the transfer of these results to actual eating behavior is problematic. While the IGT simulates situations in which the outcome of a decision is unpredictable – at least before the contingencies have been understood on the basis of feedback following previous trials – in everyday life, individuals with eating disorders most often know explicitly the deleterious long-term consequences of their disturbed eating behavior. A laboratory task that simulates decision-making with explicit rules for gains and losses and obvious probabilities is the game of dice task (GDT; Brand, Fujiwara, et al., 2005). In the GDT, individuals are required to decide between various alternatives explicitly related to a specific amount of gain or loss. Thereby, the alternatives have obvious winning probabilities. Several studies have demonstrated the GDT's sensitivity to differentiate between healthy controls and patients with a variety of clinical syndromes such as pathological gambling (Brand, Kalbe, et al., 2005), Korsakoff's syndrome (Brand, Fujiwara, et al., 2005), Parkinson's disease (Brand et al., 2004b, Euteneuer et al., 2009), Alzheimer's disease (Delazer, Sinz, Zamarian, & Benke, 2007) and selective amygdala damage (Brand, Grabenhorst, Starcke, Vandekerckhove, & Markowitsch, 2007). There is further evidence that poor decision-making in the GDT is linked to impaired capacities to advantageously utilize feedback processing and to deficits in executive function (Brand, 2008, Brand et al., 2006, Brand et al., 2008, Brand et al., 2009a, Brand et al., 2009b, Clark et al., 2008, Euteneuer et al., 2009, Labudda et al., 2008). In eating disorders, to our knowledge, only one study so far has tested decision-making processes if rules for reinforcement and punishment are explicitly clear and the outcome is defined by probabilities. Brand, Franke-Sievert, Jacoby, Markowitsch, and Tuschen-Caffier (2007) compared women with BN to a group of healthy controls in the GDT. Results revealed that, under explicit rules, patients with BN chose disadvantageous alternatives significantly more frequently than controls. Moreover, decision-making impairments were significantly related to deficits in executive functions. Several other studies indicate that women with BN have impairments in executive functions, such as attention and memory deficits (see Duchesne et al., 2004 for a review). These deficits are supposed to be linked to alterations of the serotonin system (Kaye et al., 2005) which, in turn, is thought to affect functions of the (medial) prefrontal cortex. This brain region is also involved in decision-making processes, as yielded by several studies on patients with selective damage to the ventromedial part of the prefrontal cortex (Bechara et al., 1998, Bechara et al., 2000b, Manes et al., 2002).

Contrary to the case with BN, to our knowledge, no neuropsychological studies have been conducted in BED. One study conducted by Woolley et al. (2007) allows us to draw some comparisons to individuals affected by BED. To identify the brain regions associated with binge eating, the authors compared 32 patients with neurodegenerative disease and 18 healthy control subjects in a free-feeding task using MR voxel-based morphometry (VBM). From the clinical group, those patients who binged in the task had all been previously diagnosed with frontotemporal dementia. Results using VBM revealed that binge eating patients had significantly greater atrophy in the right ventral insula, striatum and orbitofrontal cortex. Hence, in BED, functions of the prefrontal cortex may play an important role in the maintenance of binge attacks.

Overall, compared to BN and AN, little research has been conducted on cognitive biases and executive functions in BED. However, in line with a transdiagnostic approach to eating disorders (Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran, 2003), some assumptions for BED may be drawn from BN. Similarly to patients with BN, women with BED are often well aware of the deleterious consequences of ongoing binge attacks and often feel ashamed about them. Especially in periods preceding a binge attack, the ability to make advantageous decisions may be important. Clinical observations suggest that women affected by BED frequently “know” that the binge is coming and nevertheless decide to go to the supermarket to buy junk food. Or, they say that they “know” that not talking about their anger will lead to a binge attack but they still choose to say nothing. Similarly, they often know that chaotic eating patterns may trigger binge attacks but they still make attempts at restricting caloric intake at times (Masheb and Grilo, 2000, Raymond et al., 2003, Rossiter et al., 1992, Yanovski and Sebring, 1994). This is especially important, as persistent caloric restraint predicts relapse following treatment for BED (Safer, Lively, Telch, & Agras, 2002). Thus, even though other factors such as negative emotions may be the immediate trigger for the occurrence of a binge attack, decision-making processes may be of relevance in handling preceding events. We were therefore interested in analyzing decision-making processes in this group of patients.

In line with the research just mentioned, the following hypotheses were made: first, compared to healthy controls, women with BED were expected to select disadvantageous choices significantly more often. Second, women with BED were expected to show impaired capacities to advantageously utilize feedback processing compared to healthy controls. As a multidimensional construct (Guerrieri, Nederkoorn, & Jansen, 2008), impulsivity is conceptualized as a tendency to think, plan and control insufficiently (Solanto et al., 2001), which – in the case of BED – may result in the maladaptive response of bingeing. Furthermore, impulsivity has been linked with risk-taking behavior (Lejuez et al., 2002, Leland and Paulus, 2005, Nicholson et al., 2005). Therefore, our third hypothesis was that, compared to healthy controls, women with BED were expected to be characterized by an under-activation of the behavioral inhibition system and a higher activation of the behavior approach system concerning fun seeking and reward responsiveness (Gray, 1991, Gray, 1994), as measured by the BIS/BAS scale (Carver & White, 1994). Fourth, because of the correlations found between executive dysfunction and disadvantageous decision-making, we expected women with BED to score significantly worse in a task measuring cognitive flexibility. Fifth, we expected a significant positive correlation between disadvantageous decision-making and severity of eating pathology in women with BED.

Section snippets

Participants

Seventeen women with BED and 18 healthy controls (HC) participated in the study. Inclusion criterion for the BED group was the presence of a DSM-IV-TR diagnosis (APA, 2000) of BED. Exclusion criteria were the presence of substance abuse or addiction, bipolar disorder, current or past psychosis, schizophrenia, current suicidal ideation, pregnancy or lactation. As we wanted to make explicit assertions on decision-making alterations in BED, our HC were required to have a Body Mass Index (BMI = 

Results

A univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) computed with the GDT net score as dependent variable revealed that women with BED had a significant lower net score than the women in the HC group, F(1, 34) = 5.60, p = .024, effect size (Cohen's d) = 0.79 (see Fig. 1 for M and S.D.). This means that the women with BED selected the disadvantageous choices (i.e. a single number or two numbers) significantly more often than women in the HC group did. Accordingly, women with BED ended the game with a

Discussion

With the enormous availability of palatable food, the ability to decide oneself against the hedonic pleasure of eating to avoid negative long-term consequences becomes more and more important and difficult. In BED, the ability to make advantageous decisions may be especially important in periods preceding binge episodes. For example, after a period of restrained eating (Howard and Porzelius, 1999, Masheb and Grilo, 2000, Raymond et al., 2003, Rossiter et al., 1992, Yanovski and Sebring, 1994),

Acknowledgement

We thank all participants for their cooperation.

References (139)

  • M.X. Cohen et al.

    Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses

    Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research

    (2005)
  • C. Davis et al.

    Sensitivity to reward and body mass index (BMI): Evidence for a non-linear relationship

    Appetite

    (2008)
  • C. Davis et al.

    From motivation to behaviour: A model of reward sensitivity, overeating, and food preferences in the risk profile for obesity

    Appetite

    (2007)
  • C. Davis et al.

    Sensitivity to reward: Implications for overeating and overweight

    Appetite

    (2004)
  • M. Delazer et al.

    Decision-making with explicit and stable rules in mild Alzheimer's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • C.G. Fairburn et al.

    Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: A “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment

    Behavior Research and Therapy

    (2003)
  • C.E. Howard et al.

    The role of dieting in binge eating disorder: Etiology and treatment implications

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (1999)
  • J.I. Hudson et al.

    The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • J. Jamieson

    Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with difference scores

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2004)
  • W.H. Kaye et al.

    Serotonin alterations in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: New insights from imaging studies

    Physiology & Behavior

    (2005)
  • D.S. Le et al.

    Less activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in response to a meal: A feature of obesity

    American Journal of Clinical Nutrition

    (2006)
  • D.S. Le et al.

    Less activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the reanalysis of the response to a meal in obese than in lean women and its association with successful weight loss

    American Journal of Clinical Nutrition

    (2007)
  • D.S. Leland et al.

    Increased risk-taking decision-making but not altered response to punishment in stimulant-using young adults

    Drug and Alcohol Dependence

    (2005)
  • C.H. Lie et al.

    Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • P. Malloy et al.

    The orbitomedial frontal syndrome

    Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology

    (1993)
  • J. Markela-Lerenc et al.

    Prefrontal-cingulate activation during executive control: Which comes first?

    Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research

    (2004)
  • R.M. Masheb et al.

    Prognostic significance of two sub-categorization methods for the treatment of binge eating disorder: Negative affect and overvaluation predict, but do not moderate, specific outcomes

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2008)
  • O. Mobbs et al.

    Cognitive deficits and biases for food and body in bulimia: Investigation using an affective shifting task

    Eating Behavior

    (2008)
  • J.M. Mond et al.

    Recurrent binge eating with and without the “undue influence of weight or shape on self-evaluation”: Implications for the diagnosis of binge eating disorder

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2007)
  • C. Nederkoorn et al.

    Why obese children cannot resist food: The role of impulsivity

    Eating Behavior

    (2006)
  • C. Nederkoorn et al.

    Impulsivity in obese women

    Appetite

    (2006)
  • C. Nederkoorn et al.

    Restrained eaters act on impulse

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2004)
  • J.A. Perianez et al.

    Trail Making Test in traumatic brain injury, schizophrenia, and normal ageing: Sample comparisons and normative data

    Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology

    (2007)
  • APA

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (2000)
  • B. Arnow et al.

    The Emotional Eating Scale: The development of a measure to assess coping with negative affect by eating

    International Journal of Eating Disorders

    (1995)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2000)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Role of the amygdala in decision-making

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2003)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions

    Brain

    (2000)
  • A.G. Boeka et al.

    The Iowa gambling task as a measure of decision making in women with bulimia nervosa

    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society

    (2006)
  • M. Brand

    Does the feedback from previous trials influence current decisions? A study on the role of feedback processing in making decisions under explicit risk conditions

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Psychology

    (2008)
  • M. Brand et al.

    Neuropsychological correlates of decision making in patients with bulimia nervosa

    Neuropsychology

    (2007)
  • M. Brand et al.

    Decision-making deficits of korsakoff patients in a new gambling task with explicit rules: Associations with executive functions

    Neuropsychology

    (2005)
  • M. Brand et al.

    Organic and psychogenic factors leading to executive dysfunctions in a patient suffering from surgery of a colloid cyst of the Foramen of Monro

    Neurocase

    (2004)
  • M. Brand et al.

    Decision-making impairments in patients with Parkinson's disease

    Behavioral Neurology

    (2004)
  • M. Brand et al.

    Decision making with and without feedback: The role of intelligence, strategies, executive functions, and cognitive styles

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2009)
  • M. Brand et al.

    Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: Correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2007)
  • C.M. Bulik et al.

    Medical and psychiatric morbidity in obese women with and without binge eating

    International Journal of Eating Disorders

    (2002)
  • C. Carver et al.

    Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1994)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text