Skip to main content
Log in

Validation of the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition in Adolescents with ASD: Fixation Duration and Pupil Dilation as Predictors of Performance

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Impaired social cognition is one of the core characteristics of autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Appropriate measures of social cognition for high-functioning adolescents with ASD are, however, lacking. The Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC) uses dynamic social stimuli, ensuring ecological validity, and has proven to be a sensitive measure in adulthood. In the current study, 33 adolescents with ASD and 23 controls were administered the MASC, while concurrent eye tracking was used to relate gaze behavior to performance levels. The ASD group exhibited reduced MASC scores, with social cognition performance being explained by shorter fixation duration on eyes and decreased pupil dilation. These potential diagnostic markers are discussed as indicators of different processing of social information in ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 165–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. J., & Colombo, J. (2009). Larger tonic pupil size in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Psychobiology, 51(2), 207–211.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. J., Colombo, J., & Jill Shaddy, D. (2006). Visual scanning and pupillary responses in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(7), 1238–1256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403–450.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Auyeung, B., Allison, C., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Brief report: Development of the adolescent empathy and systemizing quotients. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(10), 2225–2235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in cognitive sciences, 6(6), 248–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Bowen, D. C., Holt, R. J., Allison, C., Auyeung, B., Lombardo, M. V., et al. (2015). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: Complete absence of typical Sex difference in ~400 men and women with Autism. PLoS One, 10(8), e0136521.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163–175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001a). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Spong, A., Scahill, V., & Lawson, J. (2001b). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? A test with children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5(1), 47–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, J., & Lucero-Wagoner, B. (2000). The pupillary system. Handbook of Psychophysiology, 2, 142–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaser, E., Eglington, L., Carter, A. S., & Kaldy, Z. (2014). Pupillometry reveals a mechanism for the Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) advantage in visual tasks. Scientific Reports, 4, 4301.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bölte, S., Poustka, F., & Constantino, J. N. (2008). Assessing autistic traits: Cross-cultural validation of the social responsiveness scale (SRS). Autism Research, 1(6), 354–363. doi:10.1002/aur.49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bölte, S., Rühl, D., Schmötzer, G., & Poustka, F. (2006). Diagnostisches Interview für Autismus—Revidiert. Deutsche Fassung des Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised von Michael Rutter. Ann LeCouteur und Catherine Lord. Verlag Hans Huber: Bern.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brent, E., Rios, P., Happé, F., & Charman, T. (2004). Performance of children with autism spectrum disorder on advanced theory of mind tasks. Autism, 8, 283–299. doi:10.1177/1362361304045217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. J., Shic, F., Macari, S., & Chawarska, K. (2014). Gaze response to dyadic bids at 2 years related to outcomes at 3 years in autism spectrum disorders: A subtyping analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 431–442. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3), 195–203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in cognitive sciences, 16(4), 231–239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallier, C., Parish-Morris, J., McVey, A., Rump, K. M., Sasson, N. J., Herrington, J. D., & Schultz, R. T. (2015). Measuring social attention and motivation in autism spectrum disorder using eye-tracking: Stimulus type matters. Autism Research, 8(5), 620–628. doi:10.1002/aur.1479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48, 79–93. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chuleva, S., Clausen, H.-J., & Körner, J. (2009). Anwendung des MASC, eines neuen Instrumentes zur Erfassung sozialkognitiver Kompetenzen bei Jugendlichen. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 58(8), 635–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2005). Social Responsiveness Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2007). Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2) (Vol. 2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8(4), 519–526.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • de Urabain, I. R. S., Johnson, M. H., & Smith, T. J. (2014). GraFIX: A semiautomatic approach for parsing low-and high-quality eye-tracking data. Behavior research methods, 47(1), 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Kalbe, E., Rogers, K., Hassenstab, J., Brand, M., et al. (2006). Introducing MASC: A Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 623–636. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elison, J. T., Paterson, S. J., Wolff, J. J., Reznick, J. S., Sasson, N. J., Gu, H., et al. (2013). White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(8), 899–908.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., Gliga, T., Pickles, A., Hudry, K., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., & Team, B. (2013). The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism. Behavioural Brain Research, 251, 147–154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Falck-Ytter, T. (2008). Face inversion effects in autism: A combined looking time and pupillometric study. Autism Research, 1(5), 297–306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falck-Ytter, T., Bölte, S., & Gredebäck, G. (2013a). Eye tracking in early autism research. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 5(1), 28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Falck-Ytter, T., von Hofsten, C., Gillberg, C., & Fernell, E. (2013b). Visualization and analysis of eye movement data from children with typical and atypical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 2249–2258. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1776-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2013). Social cognition: From brains to culture. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fleck, S., Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Mielke, R., Kalbe, E., & Kessler, J. (2006). Der MASC-MC, ein neues filmisches Verfahren zur Erfassung der “Theory of Mind”. Zeigen Patienten mit Asperger-Syndrom ein spezifisches Antwortmuster? Der Nervenarzt, 77, 343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, T. D. (2010). Psychometric: Applied Psychometric Theory. R package version 2.2. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psychometric.

  • Fletcher, T. D. (2012). QuantPsyc: Quantitative Psychology Tools. R package version 1.5.

  • Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Benson, V., Frank, M., & Findlay, J. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 248–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2011). An R companion to applied regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeth, M., Chapman, P., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2010). Do gaze cues in complex scenes capture and direct the attention of high functioning adolescents with ASD? Evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(5), 534–547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2008). Implicit and explicit processes in social cognition. Neuron, 60(3), 503–510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guillon, Q., Hadjikhani, N., Baduel, S., & Roge, B. (2014). Visual social attention in autism spectrum disorder: Insights from eye tracking studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 279–297. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanley, M., McPhillips, M., Mulhern, G., & Riby, D. M. (2013). Spontaneous attention to faces in Asperger syndrome using ecologically valid static stimuli. Autism, 17(6), 754–761.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 290–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(3), 194–203. doi:10.1038/35058500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jemel, B., Mottron, L., & Dawson, M. (2006). Impaired Face Processing in Autism: Fact or Artifact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 91–106. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0050-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(8), 946–954. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.8.946.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., Hatri, A., Steimke, R., & Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Atypical reflexive gaze patterns on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(37), 12281–12287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 358(1430), 345–360. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1202.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459(7244), 257–261. doi:10.1038/nature07868.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krach, S., Kamp-Becker, I., Einhäuser, W., Sommer, J., Frässle, S., Jansen, A., et al. (2015). Evidence from pupillometry and fMRI indicates reduced neural response during vicarious social pain but not physical pain in autism. Human Brain Mapping, 36(11), 4730–4744. doi:10.1002/hbm.22949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laeng, B., Sirois, S., & Gredebäck, G. (2012). Pupillometry a window to the preconscious? Perspectives on psychological science, 7(1), 18–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahera, G., Boada, L., Pousa, E., Mirapeix, I., Moron-Nozaleda, G., Marinas, L., et al. (2014). Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC): Spanish validation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(8), 1886–1896. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2061-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, F. M. (1952). The relation of the reliability of multiple-choice tests to the distribution of item difficulties. Psychometrika, 17(2), 181–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H, Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: ADOS-2. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loth, E., Spooren, W., Ham, L. M., Isaac, M. B., Auriche-Benichou, C., Banaschewski, T., et al. (2016). Identification and validation of biomarkers for autism spectrum disorders. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery, 15(1), 70–73. doi:10.1038/nrd.2015.7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martineau, J., Hernandez, N., Hiebel, L., Roché, L., Metzger, A., & Bonnet-Brilhault, F. (2011). Can pupil size and pupil responses during visual scanning contribute to the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in children? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(8), 1077–1082.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montag, C., Ehrlich, A., Neuhaus, K., Dziobek, I., Heekeren, H. R., Heinz, A., & Gallinat, J. (2010). Theory of mind impairments in euthymic bipolar patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 123(1), 264–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, D., & Spooren, W. (2012). EU-AIMS: A boost to autism research. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery, 11(11), 815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuske, H. J., Vivanti, G., Hudry, K., & Dissanayake, C. (2014). Pupillometry reveals reduced unconscious emotional reactivity in autism. Biological Psychology, 101, 24–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petermann, U., & Petermann, F. (2011). WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-: Deutschsprachige Adaptation nach D Wechsler. Amsterdam: Pearson Assessment and Information.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R., & Desmond, J. (2011). Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(1), 101–109. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poustka, L., Rehm, A., Holtmann, M., Bock, M., Böhmert, C., & Dziobek, I. (2010). Dissoziation von kognitiver und affektiver Empathie bei Jugendlichen mit Autismus-Spektrum-Störungen. Kindheit und Entwicklung., 19, 177–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preißler, S., Dziobek, I., Ritter, K., Heekeren, H. R., & Roepke, S. (2010). Social cognition in borderline personality disorder: Evidence for disturbed recognition of the emotions, thoughts, and intentions of others. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, 4(182), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prifitera, A., Saklofske, D. H., & Weiss, L. G. (2005). WISC-4 clinical use and interpretation. Burlington, MA: Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://cran.r-project.org/doc/FAQ/R-FAQ.html.

  • Raiford, S., Weiss, L., Rolfhus, E., & Coalson, D. (2005). WISC-IV technical report# 4: General Ability Index. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Revelle, W. (2015). Psych: Procedures for personality and psychological research. Evanston, Illinois: Northwestern University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, K., Moriuchi, J. M., Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2012). Parsing heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: Visual scanning of dynamic social scenes in school-aged children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(3), 238–248.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Risko, E. F., Laidlaw, K. E., Freeth, M., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Social attention with real versus reel stimuli: Toward an empirical approach to concerns about ecological validity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 143.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, K., Dziobek, I., Preißler, S., Rüter, A., Vater, A., Fydrich, T., et al. (2011). Lack of empathy in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 187(1), 241–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robin, X., Turck, N., Hainard, A., Tiberti, N., Lisacek, F., Sanchez, J. C., & Müller, M. (2011). pROC: An open-source package for R and S + to analyze and compare ROC curves. (Vol. 12).

  • Rühl, D., Bölte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., & Poustka, F. (2004). Diagnostische Beobachtungsskala für Autistische Störungen (ADOS). Bern: Verlag Hans Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Faggioli, R. (2005). ADI-R: Autism diagnostic interview–revised: Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samson, A. C., & Huber, O. W. (2010). Short German versions of empathizing and systemizing self-assessment scales. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 69, 239–244. doi:10.1024/1421-0185/a000028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, E., & Szabadi, E. (2008). Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: Its roles in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part I: Principles of functional organisation. Current Neuropharmacology, 6(3), 235.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A. (2013). Atypical development of spontaneous social cognition in autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Development, 35(2), 96–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frit, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885. doi:10.1126/science.1176170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Speer, L. L., Cook, A. E., McMahon, W. M., & Clark, E. (2007). Face processing in children with autism: Effects of stimulus contents and type. Autism, 11(3), 265–277. doi:10.1177/1362361307076925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Symeonidou, I., Dumontheil, I., Chow, W.-Y., & Breheny, R. (2015). Development of online use of theory of mind during adolescence: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.007.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, B. (2007). Effect sizes, confidence intervals, and confidence intervals for effect sizes. Psychology in the Schools, 44(5), 423–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tobii Technology. (2012). User manual–Tobii Studio. Manual Version 3.2 Rev A. 11/2012: Tobii Technology AB.

  • Torchiano, M. (2015). Effsize: Efficient Effect Size Computation. R package version 0.5.4.

  • Tseng, P.-H., Cameron, I. G., Pari, G., Reynolds, J. N., Munoz, D. P., & Itti, L. (2013). High-throughput classification of clinical populations from natural viewing eye movements. Journal of Neurology, 260(1), 275–284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland, H. (2002). Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder toward human faces: A fixation time study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 669–678. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S., Jiang, M., Duchesne, Xavier M., Laugeson, Elizabeth A., Kennedy, Daniel P., Adolphs, R., et al. (2015). Atypical visual saliency in autism spectrum disorder quantified through model-based eye tracking. Neuron, 88(3), 604–616. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.042.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wass, S. V., Jones, E. J., Gliga, T., Smith, T. J., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2015). Shorter spontaneous fixation durations in infants with later emerging autism. Scientific Reports, 5, 8284.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, A. C., Nixon, C. L., Wilson, A., & Capage, L. (1999). Social interaction skills and theory of mind in young children. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 386–391. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2004). The Wechsler intelligence scale for children-fourth edition. London: Pearson Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woerner, W., Becker, A., Friedrich, C., Rothenberger, A., Klasen, H., & Goodman, R. (2002). Normierung und Evaluation der deutschen Elternversion des Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Ergebnisse einer repräsentativen Felderhebung. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 30(2), 105–112. doi:10.1024//1422-4917.30.2.105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yi, L., Fan, Y., Quinn, P. C., Feng, C., Huang, D., Li, J., & Lee, K. (2013). Abnormality in face scanning by children with autism spectrum disorder is limited to the eye region: Evidence from multi-method analyses of eye tracking data. Journal of vision, 13(10), 5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., Yurovsky, D., & Xu, T. (2012). Visual data mining: An exploratory approach to analyzing temporal patterns of eye movements. Infancy, 17(1), 33–60. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00095.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research leading to these results has received support from the Innovative Medicines Initiative Joint Undertaking under Grant Agreement No 115300, resources of which are composed of financial contribution from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) and EFPIA companies’ in kind contribution. The current study is part of the first author’s doctoral dissertation. Parts of the results of the current study will be presented at the 9. Conference of the Society for Research in Autism Spectrum Conditions (WGAS) in Freiburg, Germany.

Author Contributions

NM conceived of the study, participated in the study's design and coordination, performed the measurement and statistical analysis, and drafted the manuscript. SB participated in its design and coordination, performed the measurement, and helped to draft the manuscript. ID participated in its design and helped to draft the manuscript. TB participated in its design and helped to draft the manuscript. LP conceived of the study, participated in its design and interpretation of the data, and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nico Müller.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Prof. Dr. Poustka received conference attendance support or was paid for public speaking by Lilly, Shire and Medice. She received research funding from EU, German Research Foundation (DFG), Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF). Prof. Dr. Dr. Banaschewski served in an advisory or consultancy role for Actelion, Hexal Pharma, Lilly, Medice, Novartis, Oxford outcomes, PCM scientific, Shire and Viforpharma. He received conference support or speaker’s fee by Medice, Novartis and Shire. He is/has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Shire & Viforpharma. Their present work is unrelated to the above grants and relationships. Prof. Dr. Isabel Dziobek receives royalties for the Swedish version of the MASC from Hans Huber Publishers. Dr. Sarah Baumeister and Nico Müller declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 16 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Müller, N., Baumeister, S., Dziobek, I. et al. Validation of the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition in Adolescents with ASD: Fixation Duration and Pupil Dilation as Predictors of Performance. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 2831–2844 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2828-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2828-z

Keywords

Navigation